Teacher question:
I teach kindergarten. Our school recently purchased the XXXX program for teaching decoding. I don’t like it as much as the program we had. One of the ways our previous program was better than XXXX is that it included pictures for each of the phonemes. The new program does not have those pictures and I think that is a real problem. Is there any evidence that I’m right that I could take to my principal? The other teachers agree with me.
Shanahan response:
I hate that question and I wish you hadn’t asked it.
Oh, sure it’s a practical question. And as a former first-grade teacher I get why you’d ask.
But it points out an error I have made in the past.
When I used to prepare primary grade teachers that question came up sometimes. And, I answered it… incorrectly.
I answered it on the basis of logic, a dangerous approach.
“Professor Shanahan is it a good idea to use pictures when you are teaching letter sounds?”
“No, impressionable young preservice teacher. It is a bad idea. Children have to learn the letters and the sounds. Adding a picture to that equation means that there is just one more thing the kids have to memorize. It is hard enough to memorize 52 letters, and 44 sounds, without having to memorize 44 pictures to go with those sounds.”
I love that answer. It makes so much logical sense. I sure sounded wise to those young women (and the occasional guy).
And, yet, I was wrong.
The research these days shows just the opposite. Using what are referred to as “embedded mnemonics,” that is pictures that remind the children of the letter sound, actually improve learning. Across various studies (Ehri, 2014; Ehri, Deffner, & Wilce, 1984; McNamara, 2012; Schmidman & Ehri, 2010) it has been found that such embedded mnemonic pictures can reduce the amount of repetition needed for kids to learn the letters and sounds, with less confusion, better long-term memory, and greater ability to transfer or apply this knowledge in reading and spelling.
Art Credit: The example of a visual mnemonic for teaching decoding was provided by the artist Cat MacInnes, from www.spelfabet.com.au/materials”
If one relies on data – rather than reasoning – the answer is kind of a no-brainer — it is a good idea to use embedded mnemonics. It looks like, at least with regard to this feature, your previous program was better than the new one.
But let’s be careful with this. What’s good in this case (the use of pictures) is not such a great idea when you are teaching words. Research has long shown that when teaching words kids do better with just the letters without any pictures.
Why would that be?
The pictures have been found to distract from the information that you need to remember a word. In that case, students don’t need a visual mnemonic -- they need to focus their attention on the combination of letters. That’s what you want them to look at. If they spend time examining the picture instead of the letters, they are less likely to learn the word.
With something as specific or one-dimensional as a letter or a phoneme, an embedded picture provides a useful mnemonic (memory support), neither distracting students from what needs to be learned or overwhelming memory. With more complex or multidimensional items it is better to focus student attention on analyzing the parts. That’s why, when I’m teaching kids to read some high frequency words, I have them look at all the letters and spell the word and try to write it from memory.
But when it comes to teaching letters and sounds, no question about it, use embedded mnemonics. They work.
And, as for answering questions about what works in reading? An instructional approach is not "best practice" just because it makes sense. That's why we use research. A lot of times, those things that are sensible are, well, not a great idea!
References
Ehri, L.C. (2014) Orthographic Mapping in the Acquisition of Sight Word Reading, Spelling Memory, and Vocabulary Learning, Scientific Studies of Reading, 18:1, 5-21, DOI: 10.1080/10888438.2013.819356
Ehri, L. C., Deffner, N. D., & Wilce, L. S. (1984). Pictorial mnemonics for phonics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(5), 880–893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.76.5.880
McNamara, G. (2012). The effectiveness of embedded picture mnemonic alphabet cards on letter recognition and letter sound knowledge. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Rowan University.
Shmidman, A., & Ehri, L. (2010) Embedded picture mnemonics to learn letters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14:2, 159-182, DOI: 10.1080/10888430903117492
Two thoughts. First, I was super lucky to have discovered Ehri's article on letter-embedded picture mnemonics before my one year teaching kindergarten, and it made a world of difference. I used a program that also involved songs and gestures. I'm not saying there's research behind those two features--I'm saying it was fun. Second, I've just finished reading Districts that Succeed: breaking the correlation between race, poverty, and achievement, and one of the superintendents featured would applaud your admission of error. He says: You have to check your ego at the door and admit there are some things you don't know. Thanks, Tim, for a very important post. I really believe we need picture mnemonics in our beginning reading programs.
You make a very good point ... that the picture clues are a great help as students are trying to learn and remember the sound (phoneme) associated with the letter. By saying the word that goes with the picture and then isolating the first sound, it provides a scaffold. However, once students have become automatic with the letter-sound correspondence the picture clue can be removed. In some phonics programs the picture is placed next to or below the letter on a letter-sound card. There are a few phonics programs, such as Telian's Lively Letters and the Letterland program that actually embed the picture into the letter shape, which seems to be added benefit, as noted in the National Reading Panel report: "The value of mnemonics for teaching letter-sound relations to kindergartners is supported by evidence. In a study by Ehri, Deffner, and Wilce (1984), children were shown letters drawn to assume the shape of a familiar object, for example, s drawn as a snake, h drawn as a house (with a chimney). Memory for the letter-sound relations was mediated by the name of the object. Children were taught to look at the letter, be reminded of the object, say its name, and isolate the first sound of the name to identify the sound (i.e., s snake - /s/). With practice they were able to look at the letters and promptly say their sounds. Children who were taught letters in this way learned them better than children who were taught letters by rehearsing the relations with pictures unrelated to the letter shapes (e.g., house drawn with a flat roof and no chimney) and also better than children who simply rehearsed the associations without any pictures. Application of this principle can be found in Letterland (Wendon, 1992), a program that teaches kindergartners letter-sound associations. In this program, all the letters are animate characters that assume the shape of the letters and have names prompting the relevant sound,for example, Sammy Snake, Hairy Hat Man, Fireman Fred, Annie Apple. The task of learning the shapes and sounds of all the alphabet letters is difficult and time consuming, particularly for children who come to school knowing none. The relations are arbitrary and meaningless. Techniques to speed up the learning process are valuable in helping kindergartners prepare for formal reading instruction. The motivational value of associating letters with interesting characters or hand motions and incorporating this into activities and games that are fun is important for promoting young children’s learning. If the task of teaching letters is stripped bare to one of memorizing letter shapes and sounds, children will become bored and easily distracted and will take much longer to learn the associations."
A more recent study also found an advantage to having picture clues over plain letters (Theresa Roberts & Carol Sadler: Letter sound characters and imaginary narratives: Can they enhance motivation and letter sound learning? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, Vol. 46, 2019). This study compared two explicit teaching approaches for teaching alphabet letter sounds to preschool children: The Letterland program (i.e., integrated mnemonics) was used for the first treatment (utilizing letters with letters sound characters integrated into the letter shapes and short narratives about the letter sound characters). In the treated control, plain letters and alphabet books were the foundation of instruction. The study found significant effects in favor of the use of Letterland on identifying letter sounds (effect size 1.31), identifying initial consonants(effect size 0.61) and blending phonemes (effect size 0.62).
The final comment I'd like to make is related to this line in the NRP report "If the task of teaching letters is stripped bare to one of memorizing letter shapes and sounds, children will become bored and easily distracted." I have observed a number of classrooms that use Letterland or Lively Letters and have seen first hand how engaged young children are when they learn basic phonics instruction in this way.
So glad Shanahan wrote about this so that this topic gets some reach! I'm surprised more phonics programs don't act on these findings. A few programs that feature letters embedded in mnemonic pictures are Letterland, ReadWriteInc, and Spelfabet. I'm sure there are others. Often you can buy just the letter cards apart from the whole program. (I've yet to find a program where I agree with all the choices for keywords -- e = egg is not the best, IMHO.)
Harriet- what pictures , songs, gestures did you uae?
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
A Question I Hate: Should We Use Pictures (Embedded Mnemonics) When Teaching Phonics?
24 commentsCopyright © 2024 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.