Recently, I heard from my colleague and friend, Claude Goldenberg. Claude is one of the most knowledgeable experts on second language literacy. He is not only intelligent, sensitive, and reliable, but someone who takes the idea of applying science to education seriously.
He noticed a problem and pointed it out to me. It was something that I had recently noticed myself in a very different context, so I was intrigued. He and I became a kind of mini “committee of correspondence”, exchanging emails about our concerns.
These days there is much attention to the science of reading in public discourse and policy making. It is aimed at instruction for both native speakers and English Learners. Claude and I are cool with that.
However, some of what is being promoted is not really science. It fits into the science drawer about as well as Kanye West fits into a Taylor Swift festival.
This blog emerged from our round robin – providing you with the wisdom of two experts for the price of one.
Sometimes an argument ends up in court. The plaintiffs and defense hire experts to support their case. Judges evaluate their expertise to determine the value of their testimony. Not every tool in the shed is sharp enough to gain “expert” status.
Journalists face a similar problem when it comes to literacy education. They usually address this by seeking a range of opinions. Expert #1 says phonics is effective and helps young children learn to read. Expert #2 disagrees.
The resulting news story concludes something like: “Educators don’t agree on the value of phonics.”
The problem with that is that most educators do not disagree over that issue.
Trying to reveal both sides of an issue is admirable.
But this kind of “on the one hand, on the other hand” reporting can make it look like a raging “reading war” when most reading scientists agree that some early phonics instruction is a good idea and should be part of any complete reading program, including those aimed at English Learners.
But what counts as expertise in this kind of kerfuffle?
Recently, I was asked about phonics by a reporter. After I told him about the instructional studies that had consistently found phonics instruction conferred a learning advantage, he expressed skepticism. He had already discussed the matter with another expert, whom he indiscreetly named. That expert assured him phonics couldn’t work.
I was gob smacked.
The named expert did know reading, but his/her expertise regarding decoding was decidedly circumscribed. This expert had never taught or supervised at the grade levels in question, had never done research on phonics or any related topics, had never prepared teachers to teach beginning reading. The study proving phonics didn’t work was an analysis of phonics generalizations from 1963 that showed exceptions to many of the “rules” then being taught!
That expert failed to note later large-scale analyses of tens of thousands of English words that found much greater spelling consistency (e.g., Venezky, 1967) and ignored all the instructional studies.
I felt bad for the journalist.
He knew neither of us. He had to evaluate our contributions based only on credentials. The other expert’s credentials were impeccable in terms of degrees, university appointments, and recognition in the field. That his/her expertise included little that would provide deep knowledge on the issue in question would not be readily apparent.
I’ve been in a similar situation. Journal editors must identify reviewers with sufficient knowledge to adequately referee research articles for publication. I would select experts with the kind of specific and specialized expertise that a decoding researcher would bring to a phonics study. However, I would also choose someone else – knowledgeable in the field more generally – to try to avoid any bias that might enter the review process due to unstated assumptions or beliefs that the more specialized reviewers might possess.
Even with that, I probably wouldn’t have chosen the same informant this journalist did; just not knowledgeable, conversant, or up to date enough to justify using him or her for an outsider’s view.
Claude pointed out a similar, less public and more in-the-weeds, disagreement between Stanislas Dehaene and Steven L. Strauss concerning the interpretation of neuroscientific research on reading. Dehaene is a noted neuroscientist with a long history of research contributions, while Strauss is a practicing neurologist with a PhD in linguistics and strong opinions about teaching reading. He neither has research credentials, nor does he appear even conversant in the neuroscientific literature that has accumulated over the past couple of decades (Dehaene, 2024). He not only has disdain for the work of pretty much every eminent scholar in that field, but rejects much of the instructional research, too. (Beware of “expert opinions” from other fields of study. A neurologist’s opinions about the best way to teach reading are about as useful as reading educators’ opinions about how best to treat neurological problems).
Advocates more interested in advancing ill-informed positions than weighing the evidence take advantage of false equivalences such as these to obscure relevant facts and neutralize relevant—but inconvenient to their position—evidence.
In a recent posting, for example, members of the California Association for Bilingual Education (CABE) wondered how “a dispute between two highly respected neuroscientists, Dr. Strauss and Dr. Dehaene” could possibly be resolved by mere mortals such as journalists or the rest of us.
Easy: Compare their credentials. This one is an easy call if we dig below the surface of competing claims. See here.
Reporters, too, wanting to surface adversarial positions (often at the behest of editors who likely know even less about the issues than they do) will report such a disagreement as if the two sides are equivalent. Then teachers and teacher educators who may themselves have little knowledge about research may cling to opinions such as Strauss’s or CABE’s to justify their own.
Misinformation among educators and advocates for English Learners (also known as Emergent Bilinguals or Multi-Lingual Learners) has especially proliferated over the past few years.
Claims have been made, for example, that these students have different brains, learn differently, and therefore require fundamentally different approaches to teaching reading. Or that reading research (aka “science of reading”) does not apply to these students because relevant studies have only included monolingual, mostly English-speaking students.
These claims have been shown to be flatly untrue in articles for a broad education audience and for those particularly interested in these populations of students. But these and other misguided beliefs can still influence unwary and uninformed educators.
Teachers and teacher educators are seemingly in the same spot as the courts and the reporters when it comes to evaluating expertise. But there is one important difference. Journalists and judges rarely have made up their minds on the issue before they start.
Professional educators often have already developed strong ideological positions about pedagogical topics, phonics instruction just being one of many.
In such cases, these educators aren’t seeking knowledge but affirmation.
Think about it: Do you believe that kids learn to read best from reading and that schools should, therefore, devote considerable time to just letting kids read on their own? Do you believe that textbooks are bad for kids? Do you think that since it is possible sometimes to guess the next word accurately without looking, there is no need to teach decoding? Do you believe that kids who speak different languages are wired differently so they need to read and learn to read differently?
If you answered yes to any of those (or to dozens of similar queries), then the question is, would any amount or type of evidence change your mind?
Over my career, I have often had to adjust my thinking because empirical studies revealed things that I did not know or believe. I consider myself to be data driven rather than ideology driven. I try to use the right kind of evidence to answer a question.
The complications and inconsistencies of English do give one pause. Maybe kids don’t need that kind of help?
Likewise, the language learning needs of English learners would seem to dwarf their decoding needs. Maybe phonics won’t help them?
Research can help answer those kinds of questions. Studies, again and again, have revealed learning benefits for such teaching – including for English Learners.
Personally, I’m less interested in opinions on this kind of issue – especially the opinions of individuals with limited knowledge on the subject in question. Journalists often seek such “experts” to provide supposedly “balanced reporting” and to humanize an issue – human voices are more appealing than research findings to most readers, whether they are educators, policy makers, or members of the public.
The problem is that one side of some arguments—such as whether phonics should be a part of reading instruction for all students, regardless of their English language proficiency—is supported by large numbers of independent studies that have been impartially reviewed. Those studies show that including phonics in a comprehensive reading program reduces failure and raises average literacy levels.
Relying on dubious expert opinion in the face of scads of actual data may not be the cause of the reading wars, but it sure keeps them going.
The solution?
Journalists should be careful of the “on the one hand, on the other hand” kind of reporting.
Real, or presumed, experts should beg off replying to journalist’s questions when they have little or no special knowledge on a topic.
Teachers and principals need to be skeptical about expert claims – especially claims made without evidence.
For a variety of reasons, none of these is easy to accomplish. But we must try to do so, and we should challenge others to do so. The alternative is continued confusion and misinformed policies and practices. Our students, their families, and our society will continue to bear the brunt.
References
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baker, S., Lesaux, N., Jayanthi, M., Dimino, J., Proctor, C. P., Morris, J., Gersten, R., Haymond, K., Kieffer, M. J., Linan-Thompson, S., & Newman-Gonchar, R. (2014). Teaching academic content and literacy to English learners in elementary and middle school (NCEE 2014-4012). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance (NCEE), Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from the NCEE website: http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/publications_reviews.aspx.
Bond, G. L., & Dykstra, R. (1967). The cooperative research program in first-grade reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 2(4), 5–142. https://doi.org/10.2307/746948
Chall, J. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: Harper & Row.
Clymer, T., & R. G. S. (1963). The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 16(4), 252–258. http://www.jstor.org/stable/20197636
Gersten, R., Baker, S.K., Shanahan, T., Linan-Thompson, S., Collins, P., & Scarcella, R. (2007). Effective Literacy and English Language Instruction for English Learners in the Elementary Grades: A Practice Guide (NCEE 2007-4011). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Gottardo, A., Chen, X., & Huo, M. R. Y. (2021). Understanding within? and cross?language relations among language, preliteracy skills, and word reading in bilingual learners: Evidence from the science of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 56, S371-S390. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.410
Li, S., & Woore, R. (2023). How Chinese learners decode L2 English words: Evidence from a phonics instruction program. Reading Research Quarterly, 58(4), 584-600. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.515
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: Reports of the Subgroups. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.
Venezky, R. L. (1967). English orthography: Its graphical structure and its relation to sound. Reading Research Quarterly, 2(3), 75–105. https://doi.org/10.2307/747031
Washburn, E. K., Gesel, S. A., Fitzgerald, M. S., Beach, K. D., & Kingsbery, C. R. (2023). The impact of a comprehensive, evidence-based approach to summer literacy intervention on the K-3 reading skills of economically and culturally diverse students. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 39(6), 510-529. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2022.2147463
LISTEN TO MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Podcast
Tim, my other field (well, one of...) has a way of handling these conflicting views. You know it as 'Open Source', though readers may have a limited view of what an open source community really is.
One key to open source work is change tracking. As opposed to research journals, which have a hard-to-navigate trail of evidence, a particular statement or line of code in open source software has a transparent chain of change, going back to the original creation, or 'commit' of that item.
We're trying to build such a transparent chain of evidence for the most critical aspects of teaching literacy.
It's still very early. We'll have to assemble many community members to make it work.
Here's the public-facing version: https://sorquizzes.org
The best examples of where we're heading is under the 'Basic' category.
The change trail will be kept on the GitHub platform: https://github.com/EdJones/sor-quizzes/tree/main/src/data
Anyone can and should contribute.
Once again Dr Shanahan you have provided a much needed voice of reason. That is why your blog is included in my syllabi!
Timely posting with so many applications for today and democratic societies.
Have you read with Kindergarten and 1st grade readers in their classrooms or 1:1 since 2023-2024. I have done both and also tutor early readers and/or struggling readers
I am Not a certified researcher.
I am just an adjunct professor who supervises Elementary and Sped Ed aspiring teachers/student teachers.
Prior, I was/am a certified Reading Specialist in the state of VA and a certified SPED Teacher. Yes, I was certified in literacy coaching and RR. I worked in a county prior to recently retiring from pubic education writing curriculum as part of county mandates in reading and writing for mostly K-2.
I am personally appalled at what I see
happening in primary classrooms. I have sat down next to these young readers to read with them and I assess readers free of charge for parents with concerns about their child. Sometimes I will tutor children depending on my schedule.
I have also talked with classroom teachers many times and I have observed teaching of phonics lessons and all other LA whole group and small group instruction.
To clarify, I fully agree with and support the teaching of phonics. I encourage my readers to attend to graphophonemic information and check it with semantics and syntax. I furthermore believe that even the youngest reader should be able to tell me about the page they read and/or the book.
I see less and less evidence of this in the three northern VA counties I work. Instead, I see children using symbols and sounds for almost every word they read, even if the word or name is repeated on a page.
I find this SCARY and against my understanding of reading. I am seeing this at all early grade levels through 4th grade. I find this more than random. And I would love to know what you are seeing and who is doing research in classrooms currently to understand what LA instruction looks like now, what promoting is occurring when teachers are listening to readers read one on one, assessments being used and the impact on high stakes state testing for I believe these tests Will be impacted.
Thank you.
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2025 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.