Are We Teaching Reading Comprehension? Part II – 6 Things Every Teacher Should Know

  • challenging text comprehension strategies text complexity executive function reading comprehension
  • 29 March, 2025
  • 12 Comments

Previously, I discussed two observational studies (a landmark from the 1970s and a valuable recent effort). Both aimed to determine the amount of reading comprehension instruction in American schools. That’s an endeavor that requires clear definitions.

According to the older study conducted by Dolores Durkin (1978), comprehension instruction included any action teachers took to help children “understand or work out the meaning of more than a single, isolated word.” The newer study, this one by Philip Capin and colleagues (2024), didn’t provide a definition as much as a list of acceptable actions: teaching word meaning knowledge, developing background knowledge, selecting texts to match lesson goals, establishing engaging and motivational contexts for reading, engaging students in collaborative learning, teaching comprehension strategies, teaching text structure, and high-quality discussions of text content.

Durkin explicitly rejected the post-reading question-and-answer sessions so common in schools. To her, that was assessment only, not instruction. Capin accepted the value of high-quality discussion but distinguished that from the more typical quiz show routines that accompany reading.

RELATED: Are We Teaching Reading Comprehension? Part I

Neither study homed in on the most essential feature of comprehension teaching. It is insufficient to enable students to understand a story or article. If the aim isn’t to alter reading behaviors for future texts, then it isn’t comprehension instruction. Reading practice is not enough.

Here are 6 important things that everyone should know about comprehension teaching.

1.        Amount of comprehension instruction

Both studies claimed there was not enough comprehension instruction. By Durkin’s definition, there was almost zero such teaching, though it increased considerably when those Q&A sessions were counted (Hodges, 1980). Capin found that 23% of reading instruction focused on comprehension.

I’ve long argued for at least 2 hours per day for reading/writing/spelling/language instruction, with a quarter of that devoted to comprehension.

Given that, I don’t think our problem is too little comprehension time. No, it’s that almost any kind of reading practice is claimed to build comprehension. Teachers have kids read texts. They ask questions to be sure the kids understood those texts. They might even do some other things – teaching the meanings of some of the words, providing relevant background information, and so on. Those actions are almost certain to ensure comprehension of the instructional text, but let’s be honest. They have little likelihood of any generalizable impact.  

2.        The centrality of text

The Capin study credited the value of using texts relevant to the purposes of instruction. That inclusion deserves a hug!

A good deal of reading comprehension instruction should take the form of directed reading – with a teacher guiding a group of students through a shared text. These communally read texts determine what can be taught. Given the wide range of things that readers need to learn, these texts must do double and triple duty. Too often they are chosen to support narrow aspects of the curriculum.

For instance, publishers are scrambling to ensure that reading texts expose kids to lots of information. Knowledge is in right now.

But there is more to it than that.

I’m happy to see science and history selections in the reading books. These subjects have been too long ignored. Unfortunately, these programs seem to espouse the idea that social studies and science provide “knowledge,” but literature does not. That’s a big mistake.

However, even if stories and poetry are in, knowledge should not be the only concern when selecting texts for comprehension instruction.

Students should learn to read narratives, expositions, and arguments, and to surmount the varied demands of different genres, rhetorical structures, and text conventions. Children need to be reading books that expose them to those kinds of text features and formats, and they should receive instructional guidance on how to read them.

Another important consideration is the challenge level of these instructional texts. Teachers have been told to teach with books at the students’ instructional levels, texts that could be read with 75-89% comprehension with no teacher support. The idea of that has been to ensure comprehension, but it is comprehension of texts the students can already read reasonably well.  

That’s just the opposite of what is needed (Shanahan, 2025). Kids should be grappling with books they cannot read well, and instruction should help transform those into something comprehensible, without the teacher reading the books to the kids or telling them what they say.

3.        Language comprehension

You can’t tell from either the Durkin or Capin studies – or from lots of other work on reading comprehension – that language matters. Durkin specifically rejects vocabulary instruction as comprehension pedagogy, while Capin et al. includes both vocabulary and text structure.

Much vocabulary instruction focuses on teaching students the meanings of words from the instructional texts, efforts mainly aimed at ensuring success with those texts. That would seem too narrow to count as comprehension instruction by my definition. However, it doesn’t matter why students are learning word meanings, since any word could come up in a future text.

I would count such vocabulary teaching as part of comprehension, though to be fair these lessons are often conceived and delivered so poorly that perhaps it shouldn’t count as teaching at all. For one thing, introducing those words is not likely to lead to long term learning. Any vocabulary that is introduced, needs to be reviewed frequently and kids need to be induced to use these words in their talking and writing.

Another problem with this pre-teaching is that much of it is useless. The words selected for teaching may be defined explicitly in the text. Instead of guiding kids to recognize and make sense of the definitions in the text (reading comprehension), the preteaching encourages kids to ignore the text.

The same can be said for words that may not be defined explicitly but that could be figured out from context, morphology, or the use of a dictionary. Telling those word meanings may discourage the use of those essential tools and won’t teach students how to use them.  

Of course, language depends on more than words. Words are organized into sentences and there are rules or conventions that dictate relationships among words. Formal grammar instruction may not improve reading comprehension, but there is extensive research showing that kids struggle to understand complex sentences and that applied grammar instruction (e.g., guiding students to figure out the meaning of sentences) can improve it.

The same can be said about cohesion. To comprehend text, readers must track the relationships of ideas across text (e.g., pronouns, synonyms, conjunctions). Cohesion can be an important source of confusion, and it can be taught profitably. Text organization is another way that ideas connect across text, and Capin and company wisely included this as a component of comprehension instruction.

I don’t know if any of these observational studies saw such teaching of language, but it is clear from scads research that they should have beent.

4.        The Development of Knowledge

Research shows that reading comprehension depends on knowledge. Readers use knowledge to make sense of the information in texts. The knowledge a reader brings to a text can reduce the cognitive load required to comprehend, it can be the source of inferences, and it can help with long term recall of the information gained from the text.

Research shows that getting kids to think about what they already know about the text topic before they read, or increasing what the students know about the topic, can improve comprehension of that text.

But does it have a general impact on reading comprehension? That is less clear.

Unfortunately, research studies do not yet show wide transferability, though some recent research (Kim, 2024) suggests that it may be possible – but that takes more than just accumulating bits of information.

“Knowledge oriented” reading programs so far do not appear to be especially effective. This may be like vocabulary, however. Vocabulary teaching can have a positive impact on the comprehension of any texts that use those particular words. Building knowledge on certain topics within reading lessons may improve future comprehension with texts on those topics, but without a more general payoff.

I think reading lessons should aim to ensure that the students gain content from the texts used for reading comprehension. That means both ensuring that the texts used for this teaching are rich and informative, and that there is sufficient reading, discussion, and writing about those ideas to ensure learning. Nevertheless, at this point, building knowledge, per se, should not count as reading comprehension instruction. 

5.        Executive Function

Executive function refers to processes used to manage or direct our cognition. Executive function allows you to focus your attention on one thing and not another or to remember something and ignore something else. Reading comprehension like any cognitive activity falls under the control of this executive function.

There are many ways that instruction can address this: teaching students to try to comprehend (increasing intentionality) and how to comprehend (how to intentionally take comprehension supportive actions), and how to monitor success (recognizing when they’re comprehending and when they’re not, and how to fix that).

There is an extensive body of research that shows that a small amount of this kind of teaching and guidance can have powerful impacts on comprehension and learning from text. Students should be taught strategies for making sense of text, and this kind of teaching should count as comprehension instruction.

The Capin study found that such teaching was not uncommon, but that its quality was questionable. Too often this teaching is aimed only at the instructional text. Teachers are not guiding kids to use these strategies independently. The teaching too often fails to provide the conditional knowledge needed for students to be able to use it on their own. (Unfortunately, the arguments between knowledge and strategy advocates have drawn a line in the sand. Schools, instead of trying to ensure the quality of their strategy teaching are trying to replace it with an emphasis on information alone.)

Executive function instruction to be effective must be delivered with texts hard enough to require strategic effort. The teaching then should both help students to surmount the problems in the instructional texts and should offer counsel as to how to recognize these same barriers in the future and how to take those on independently.

6.        Questioning and answering

These observational studies spurned the questioning routines so common to classroom comprehension lessons.

That doesn’t mean teachers shouldn’t ask questions, only that the questions need to be framed differently, and they need to lead to… teaching and learning.

Monitoring student comprehension should be more purposeful than it usually is. Think of the language teaching noted above. Asking questions aimed at revealing whether students figured out the meaning of an undefined word, or of a complicated sentence, or of a subtle cohesive relationship may uncover the need for comprehension guidance.

Too many teachers and principals have supposed that asking questions like the ones on their state tests will lead to higher reading comprehension scores, and despite the research they don’t want to be convinced otherwise.

The problem with this approach is that practice with question formats doesn’t generalize. This teaching might inform me that future tests might ask main idea questions, but it doesn’t help me to figure out the main idea of a text, since there is neither a widely accepted definition of main idea, nor do authors convey their main idea in any standard manner. Such practice just can’t work.

That doesn’t mean that there are not questioning routines that can steer improved comprehension and learning. For instance, an important thing to watch for when reading stories are clues to the characters’ motivations. Students can learn to identify each character’s goals and the actions that they take to accomplish those goals. Similarly, with history text, students might learn to create timelines for the various events; in other words, paying attention to the years and dates.

The reason why these kinds of questioning routines can be useful is because they cue students to seek information that is identifiable in many texts. If the questions focus on that kind of information, then their impacts can be generalizable. Those kinds of questions tell students what we think is important. Add to this some teacher explanations about the value of seeking that kind of information and how to identify it, and you have some pretty good comprehension teaching.

I agree with the Capin study that there is more comprehension instruction than Durkin reported. But also agree with them that the quality of what is being delivered in the name of comprehension is woefully anemic.

Text and sufficient text challenge need to play much bigger roles in comprehension teaching. Such texts allow for the conveyance of richer content which increases the possibility of knowledge building. Challenging texts also increase both the importance and the possibility of learning when it comes to language acquisition and strategy learning.


READ MORE: Shanahan on Literacy Blogs

References

Adlof, S. M., & Catts, H. W. (2015). Morphosyntax in poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28(7), 1051-1070.

Al Otaiba, S., Connor, C. M., & Crowe, E. (2018). Promise and feasibility of teaching expository text structure: A primary grade pilot study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(9), 1997-2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9769-6

Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp.  77–117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Anderson, R. C., Pichert, J. W., & Shirey, L. L. (1983). Effects of the reader's schema at different points in time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(2), 271-279. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.75.2.271

Baker, D. L., Alberto, P. C., Macaya, M. M., García, I., & Gutiérrez-Ortega, M. (2022). Relation between the essential components of reading and reading comprehension in monolingual Spanish-speaking children: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 34(4), 2661-2696. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-022-09694-1

Baldwin, R. S., Peleg-Bruckner, Z., & McClintock, A. H. (1985). Effects of topic interest and prior knowledge on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(4), 497-504. https://doi.org/10.2307/747856

Baretta, L., Tomitch, L., MacNair, N., Lim, V. K., & Waldie, K. E. (2009). Inference making while reading narrative and expository texts: An ERP study. Psychology & Neuroscience, 2(2), 137-145. https://doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.005

Barth, A. E., & Elleman, A. (2017). Evaluating the impact of a multistrategy inference intervention for middle-grade struggling readers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48(1), 31-41. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_LSHSS-16-0041

Balthazar, C. H., & Scott, C. M. (2007). Syntax-morphology. In A. G. Kamhi, J. J. Masterson & K. Apel (Eds.), Clinical decision making in developmental language disorders; clinical decision making in developmental language disorders (pp. 143-163). Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. London: Cambridge University Press.

Baumann, J. F. (1986). Teaching third-grade students to comprehend anaphoric relationships: The application of a direct instruction model. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(1), 70-90. https://doi.org/10.2307/747961

Beck, I.L., Perfetti, C.A., & McKeown, M.G. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology74(4), 506–521. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.4.506

Best, R. M., Rowe, M., Ozuru, Y. & McNamara, D. S. (2005). Deep-Level Comprehension of Science Texts. Topics in Language Disorders, 25 (1), 65-83.

Best, R. M., Floyd, R. G., & McNamara, D. S. (2008). Differential competencies contributing to children's comprehension of narrative and expository texts. Reading Psychology, 29(2), 137-164. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710801963951

Black, E. L. (1954). The difficulties of training college students in understanding what they read. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 24, 17-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1954.tb02873.x

Bogaerds-Hazenberg, S., Evers-Vermeul, J., & Bergh, H. (2021). A meta?analysis on the effects of text structure instruction on reading comprehension in the upper elementary grades. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(3), 435-462. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.311

Booth, J. R., Harasaki, Y., & Burman, D. D. (2006). Development of lexical and sentence level context effects for dominant and subordinate word meanings of homonyms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35(6), 531-554. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-006-9028-5

Boscolo, P., Gelati, C., & Galvan, N. (2012). Teaching elementary school students to play with meanings and genre. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 28(1), 29-50. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2012.632730

Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 144-179. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654309359353

Bowey, J. A. (1986). Syntactic awareness and verbal performance from preschool to fifth grade. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15(4), 285-308.

Bowey, J. A., & Patel, R. K. (1988). Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9(4), 367-383.

Bratlie, S. S., Brinchmann, E. I., Melby-Lervåg, M., & Torkildsen, J. v. K. (2022). Morphology—A gateway to advanced language: Meta-analysis of morphological knowledge in language-minority children. Review of Educational Research, 92(4), 614-650. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543211073186

Breznitz, Z. (2005). Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. New York: Routledge.

Brimo, D., Apel, K., & Fountain, T. (2017). Examining the contributions of syntactic awareness and syntactic knowledge to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 40(1), 57-74.

Brimo, D., Lund, E., & Sapp, A. (2018). Syntax and reading comprehension: A meta?analysis of different spoken?syntax assessments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 53(3), 431-445.

Burkart, K. H. (1945). An analysis of reading abilities. The Journal of Educational Research, 38, 430-439. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1945.10881362

Cabell, S. Q., & Hwang, H. (2020). Building content knowledge to boost comprehension in the primary grades. Reading Research Quarterly, 55, S99-S107. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.338

Cabell, S. Q., Kim, J. S., White, T. G., Gale, C. J., Edwards, A. A., Hwang, H., . . . Raines, R. M. (2025). Impact of a content-rich literacy curriculum on kindergarteners’ vocabulary, listening comprehension, and content knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 117(2), 153. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000916

Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 679-694.

Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 683-696.

Capin, P., Dahl-Leonard, K., Hall, C., Yoon, N. Y., Cho, E., Chatzoglou, E., Reiley, S., Walker, M., Shanahan, E., Andress, T., & Vaughn, S. (2025). Reading comprehension instruction: Evaluating our progress since Durkin’s seminal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 29(1), 85–114. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2024.2418582

Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 278-293.

Chandy, R., Serrano, R., & Pellicer-Sánchez, A. (2025). The effect of context on the processing and learning of novel l2 vocabulary while reading. Applied Psycholinguistics. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716424000407

Chilton, M. W., & Ehri, L. C. (2015). Vocabulary learning: Sentence contexts linked by events in scenarios facilitate third graders’ memory for verb meanings. Reading Research Quarterly, 50(4), 439-458.

Choi, W., Tong, X., & Cain, K. (2016). Lexical prosody beyond first-language boundary: Chinese lexical tone sensitivity predicts English reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 70-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.04.002

Clark, M. K., & Kamhi, A. G. (2014). Influence of prior knowledge and interest on fourth- and fifth-grade passage comprehension on the qualitative reading Inventory—4. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(4), 291. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_LSHSS-13-0074

Clinton, V., Taylor, T., Bajpayee, S., Davison, M. L., Carlson, S. E., & Seipel, B. (2020). Inferential comprehension differences between narrative and expository texts: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 33(9), 2223-2248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10044-2

Colenbrander, D., von Hagen, A., Kohnen, S., Wegener, S., Ko, K., Beyersmann, E., . . . Castles, A. (2024). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy outcomes for children in English-speaking countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 36(4), 119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09953-3

Cruz Neri, N., Bernholt, S., Härtig, H., Schmitz, A., & Retelsdorf, J. (2024). Cognitive and motivational characteristics as predictors of students’ expository versus narrative text comprehension. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 39(2), 885-905. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-023-00717-1

Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 277-299.

Cutting, L. E., Materek, A., Cole, C. A. S., Levine, T. M., & Mahone, E. M. (2009). Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 34-54.

Deacon, S. H., & Kieffer, M. (2018). Understanding how syntactic awareness contributes to reading comprehension: Evidence from mediation and longitudinal models. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(1), 72-86.

De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & Van De Ven, A. (2001). Improving text comprehension strategies in upper primary school children: A design experiment. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(4), 531-559. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709901158668

Dearborn, W. F., Johnston, P. W., & Carmichael, L. (1952). Psychological writing, easy and hard for whom? American Psychologist, 7(6), 195-196. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0061566

Duran, N. D., McCarthy, P. M., Graesser, A. C., & McNamara, D. S. (2007). Using temporal cohesion to predict temporal coherence in narrative and expository texts. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 212-223. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193150

Durkin, D. (1978). What Classroom Observations Reveal about Reading Comprehension Instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14(4), 481–533. http://www.jstor.org/stable/747260

Elleman, A.M. (2017). Examining the impact of inference instruction on the literal and inferential comprehension of skilled and less skilled readers: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(6), 761–781. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu00 00180

Elleman, A.M., Lindo, E.J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D.L. (2009). The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage-level comprehension of school-age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness2(1), 1–44. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/19345740802539200

Elleman, A. M., Olinghouse, N. G., Gilbert, J. K., Spencer, J. L., & Compton, D. L. (2017). Developing content knowledge in struggling readers: Differential effects of strategy instruction for younger and older elementary students. The Elementary School Journal, 118(2), 232-256. https://doi.org/10.1086/694322

Elleman, A. M., Oslund, E. L., Griffin, N. M., & Myers, K. E. (2019). A review of middle school vocabulary interventions: Five research-based recommendations for practice. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50(4), 477-492. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-VOIA-18-0145

Escobar, J., & Espinoza, V. (2024). Direct and indirect effects of inhibition, working memory and cognitive flexibility on reading comprehension of narrative and expository texts: Same or different effects? Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2024.2400993

Farnia, F., & Geva, E. (2013). Growth and predictors of change in English language learners’ reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(4), 389-421.

Freebody, P., & Anderson, R. C. (1983). Effects of vocabulary difficulty, text cohesion, and schema availability on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 18(3), 277-294. https://doi.org/10.2307/747389

Fukkink, R.G., & de Glopper, K. (1998). Effects of instruction in deriving word meaning from context: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 68(4), 450–469.

Gallini, J. K., Spires, H. A., Terry, S., & Gleaton, J. (1993). The influence o macro and micro-level cognitive strategies training on text learning. Journal of Research & Development in Education, 26(3), 164-178.

Gallini, J. K., Spires, H. A., Terry, S., & Gleaton, J. (1993). The influence of macro and micro-level cognitive strategies training on text learning. Journal of Research & Development in Education, 26(3), 164-178.

García-Madruga, J. A., Elosúa, M. R., Gil, L., Gómez-Veiga, I., Vila, J. Ó., Orjales, I., . . . Duque, G. (2013). Reading comprehension and working memory's executive processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(2), 155-174. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.44

García-Sánchez, J., & García-Martín, J. (2021). Cognitive strategies and textual genres in the teaching and evaluation of advanced reading comprehension (ARC). Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.723281

Gasparinatou, A., & Grigoriadou, M. (2013). Exploring the effect of background knowledge and text cohesion on learning from texts in computer science. Educational Psychology, 33(6), 645-670. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2013.790309

Gernsbacher, M. A., Hallada, B. M., & Robertson, R. R. W. (1998). How automatically do readers infer fictional characters' emotional states? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(3), 271-300. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0203_5

Gillam, S. L., Vaughn, S., Roberts, G., Capin, P., Fall, A., Israelsen-Augenstein, M., . . . Gillam, R. B. (2023). Improving oral and written narration and reading comprehension of children at-risk for language and literacy difficulties: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Educational Psychology, 115(1), 99-117. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000766

Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions: Effects on literacy achievement of children with literacy difficulties. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 60(2), 183-208. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0041-x

Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions in english: Effects on literacy outcomes for school-age children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(4), 257-285. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.689791

Goodwin, A. P., Petscher, Y., & Reynolds, D. (2021). Unraveling adolescent language & reading comprehension: The monster’s data. Scientific Studies of Reading.

Gottardo, A., Mirza, A., Koh, P. W., Ferreira, A., & Javier, C. (2018). Unpacking listening comprehension: The role of vocabulary, morphological awareness, and syntactic knowledge in reading comprehension. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(8), 1741-1764.

Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., Cai, Z., Conley, M., Li, H., & Pennebaker, J. (2014). Coh-metrix measures text characteristics at multiple levels of language and discourse. Elementary School Journal, 115(2), 210-229. https://doi.org/10.1086/678293

Graesser, A.C., McNamara, D.S., & Kulikowich, J.M. (2011). Coh-Metrix providing multilevel analyses of text characteristics. Educational Researcher, 40(5), 223–234.

Graham, S., & Hebert, M. (2011). Writing to read: A meta-analysis of the impact of writing and writing instruction on reading. Harvard Educational Review, 81(4), 710-744. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.81.4.t2k0m13756113566

Guerrero, T. A., & Wiley, J. (2021). Expecting to teach affects learning during study of expository texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(7), 1281-1303. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000657

Hagtvet, B. E. (2003). Listening comprehension and reading comprehension in poor decoders: Evidence for the importance of syntactic and semantic skills as well as phonological skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16(6), 505-539.

Hall, C., Capin, P., Vaughn, S., Gillam, S. L., Wada, R., Fall, A., . . . Gillam, R. B. (2021). Narrative instruction in elementary classrooms: An observation study. The Elementary School Journal, 121(3), 454-483. https://doi.org/10.1086/712416

Hall, S. S., Kowalski, R., Paterson, K. B., Basran, J., Filik, R., & Maltby, J. (2015). Local text cohesion, reading ability and individual science aspirations: Key factors influencing comprehension in science classes. British Educational Research Journal, 41(1), 122-142. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3134

Halliday, M. A., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Routledge.

Hattan, C., Alexander, P. A., & Lupo, S. M. (2024). Leveraging what students know to make sense of texts: What the research says about prior knowledge activation. Review of Educational Research, 94(1), 73-111. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543221148478

Hebert, M., Bohaty, J. J., Nelson, J. R., & Brown, J. (2016). The effects of text structure instruction on expository reading comprehension: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(5), 609-629. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000082

Hirschman, M. (2000). Language repair via metalinguistic means. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(2), 251-268.

Hodges, C. A. (1980). Commentary: Toward a Broader Definition of Comprehension Instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 15(2), 299–306. https://doi.org/10.2307/747330

Hunt, J. T. (1953). The relation among vocabulary, structural analysis, and reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 44(4), 193-202. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056328

Hunt, L. C., Jr. (1957). Can we measure specific factors associated with reading comprehension? The Journal of Educational Research, 51, 161-172. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1957.10882453

James, W. (1890). Psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co.

Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 719-729.

Jiang, H., & Logan, J. (2019). Improving reading comprehension in the primary grades: Mediated effects of a language-focused classroom intervention. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(8), 2812-2828.

Kesler, T., Gibson, L., Jr., & Turansky, C. (2016). Bringing the book to life: Responding to historical fiction using digital storytelling. Journal of Literacy Research, 48(1), 39-79. https://doi.org/10.1177/1086296X16654649

Kiili, C., Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., Ruotsalainen, J., & Räikkönen, E. (2024). Reading comprehension skills and prior topic knowledge serve as resources when adolescents justify the credibility of multiple online texts. Reading Psychology, 45(7), 662. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2024.2351485

Kim, J. S., Burkhauser, M. A., Mesite, L. M., Asher, C. A., Relyea, J. E., Fitzgerald, J., & Elmore, J. (2021). Improving reading comprehension, science domain knowledge, and reading engagement through a first-grade content literacy intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(1), 3-26. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000465

Kim, J. S., Burkhauser, M. A., Relyea, J. E., Gilbert, J. B., Scherer, E., Fitzgerald, J., . . . McIntyre, J. (2023). A longitudinal randomized trial of a sustained content literacy intervention from first to second grade: Transfer effects on students’ reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 115(1), 73-98. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000751

Kim, J. S., Gilbert, J. B., Relyea, J. E., Rich, P., Scherer, E., Burkhauser, M. A., & Tvedt, J. N. (2024). Time to transfer: Long-term effects of a sustained and spiraled content literacy intervention in the elementary grades. Developmental Psychology, 60(7), 1279-1297. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001710

Kim, J. S., Relyea, J. E., Burkhauser, M. A., Scherer, E., & Rich, P. (2021). Improving elementary grade students’ science and social studies vocabulary knowledge depth, reading comprehension, and argumentative writing: A conceptual replication. Educational Psychology Review, 33(4), 1935-1964. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-021-09609-6

Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children's morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(2), 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5

Kostons, D., & van der WerfWerf, G. (2015). The effects of activating prior topic and metacognitive knowledge on text comprehension scores. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 264-275. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12069

Kraal, A., van den Broek, Paul W., Koornneef, A. W., Ganushchak, L. Y., & Saab, N. (2019). Differences in text processing by low- and high-comprehending beginning readers of expository and narrative texts: Evidence from eye movements. Learning and Individual Differences, 74, 14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.101752

Lauterbach, S. L., & Bender, W. N. (1995). Cognitive strategy instruction for reading comprehension: A success for high school freshmen. High School Journal, 79(1), 58-64.

Lawrence, J. F., Hagen, A. M., Hwang, J. K., Lin, G., & Lervåg, A. (2019). Academic vocabulary and reading comprehension: Exploring the relationships across measures of vocabulary knowledge. Reading and Writing32(2), 285–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9865-2

Lawrence, J. F., Knoph, R., McIlraith, A., Kulesz, P. A., & Francis, D. J. (2022). Reading comprehension and academic vocabulary: Exploring relations of item features and reading proficiency. Reading Research Quarterly57(2), 669–690. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.434

Lever, R., & Sénéchal, M. (2010). Discussing stories: On how a dialogic reading intervention improves kindergartners’ oral narrative construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(1), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.002

Lever, R., & Sénéchal, M. (2010). Discussing stories: On how a dialogic reading intervention improves kindergartners’ oral narrative construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(1), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.002

List, A. (2021). Investigating the cognitive affective engagement model of learning from multiple texts: A structural equation modeling approach. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(4), 781-817. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.361

List, A., & Sun, Y. (2023). To clarity and beyond: Situating higher-order, critical, and critical-analytic thinking in the literature on learning from multiple texts. Educational Psychology Review, 35(2), 63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-023-09756-y

Liu, H. (2021). Does questioning strategy facilitate second language (L2) reading comprehension? the effects of comprehension measures and insights from reader perception. Journal of Research in Reading, 44(2), 339-359. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12339

Liu, H., Hu, Z., & Peng, D. (2013). Evaluating word in phrase: The modulation effect of emotional context on word comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42(4), 379-391. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-012-9221-7

MacKay, E., Lynch, E., Sorenson Duncan, T., & Deacon, S. H. (2021). Informing the science of reading: Students’ awareness of sentence-level information is important for reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly.

MacLean, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1989). The processing of cohesion in fiction and non-fiction by good and poor readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 12(1), 13-28. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.1989.tb00300.x

Maguet, M. L., Morrison, T. G., Wilcox, B., & Billen, M. T. (2021). Improving children’s reading comprehension by teaching inferences. Reading Psychology, 42(3), 264-280. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2021.1888351

McCormick, S., & Hill, D. S. (1984). An analysis of the effects of two procedures for increasing disabled readers' inferencing skills. The Journal of Educational Research, 77(4), 219-226. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1984.10885527

McCrudden, M. T., Kulikowich, J. M., Lyu, B., & Huynh, L. (2022). Promoting integration and learning from multiple complementary texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114(8), 1832-1843. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000746

McKeown, M.G., Beck, I.L., Omanson, R.C., & Perfetti, C.A. (1983). The effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on reading comprehension: A replication. Journal of Reading Behavior,  15(1),  3–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862968309547474

McNamara, D. S. (2010). Strategies to read and learn: Overcoming learning by consumption. Medical Education, 44(4), 340-346. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03550.x

McNamara, D. S., Crossley, S. A., & McCarthy, P. M. (2010). Linguistic features of writing quality. Written Communication, 27(1), 57-86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741088309351547

Meyer, B. J. F., Wijekumar, K., & Lei, P. (2018). Comparative signaling generated for expository texts by 4th–8th graders: Variations by text structure strategy instruction, comprehension skill, and signal word. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(9), 1937-1968. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9871-4

Mokhtari, K., & Thompson, H. B. (2006). How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth-graders. Reading Research and Instruction, 46(1), 73-94.

Morris, R.D., Lovett, M.W., Wolf, M., Sevcik, R.A., Steinbach, K.A., Frijters, J.C., & Shapiro, M.B. (2012). Multiple component remediation for developmental reading disabilities: IQ, socioeconomic status, and race as factors in remedial outcome. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(2), 99–127.

Mosher, D. M., Burkhauser, M. A., & Kim, J. S. (2024). Improving second-grade reading comprehension through a sustained content literacy intervention: A mixed-methods study examining the mediating role of domain-specific vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 116(4), 550-568. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000868

Mosher, D. M., & Kim, J. S. (2025). Building a science of teaching reading and vocabulary: Experimental effects of structured supplements for a read aloud lesson on third graders’ domain-specific reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 29(1), 7. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2024.2368145

Nagy, W.E., Anderson, R.C., & Herman, P.A. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal24(2), 237–270. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312024002237

Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Factors influencing syntactic awareness skills in normal readers and poor comprehenders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(2), 229-241.

Neville, D. D., & Searls, E. F. (1991). A meta-analytic review of the effect of sentence-combining on reading comprehension. Reading Research and Instruction, 31(1), 63-76.

Nippold, M.A. (2017). Reading comprehension deficits in adolescents: Addressing underlying language abilities. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48(2), 125-131

Nomvete, P., & Easterbrooks, S. R. (2020). Phrase-reading mediates between words and syntax in struggling adolescent readers. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 41(3), 162-175.

Ozuru, Y., Dempsey, K., & McNamara, D. S. (2009). Prior knowledge, reading skill, and text cohesion in the comprehension of science texts. Learning and Instruction, 19(3), 228-242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.04.003

Pany, D., Jenkins, J.R., & Schreck, J. (1982). Vocabulary instruction: Effects on word knowledge and reading comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly5(3), 202–215. https://doi.org/10.2307/1510288

Pichert, J. W., & Anderson, R. C. (1977). Taking different perspectives on a story. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(4), 309-315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.69.4.309

Pickren, S. E., Stacy, M., Del Tufo, S. N., Spencer, M., & Cutting, L. E. (2022). The contribution of text characteristics to reading comprehension: Investigating the influence of text emotionality. Reading Research Quarterly, 57(2), 649-667. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.431

Poulsen, M., Nielsen, J. L., & Vang Christensen, R. (2022). Remembering sentences is not all about memory: Convergent and discriminant validity of syntactic knowledge and its relationship with reading comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 49(2), 349-365.

Priebe, S. J., Keenan, J. M., & Miller, A. C. (2012). How prior knowledge affects word identification and comprehension. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(1), 131-149. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9260-0

Proctor, C. P., Silverman, R. D., Harring, J. R., Jones, R. L., & Hartranft, A. M. (2020). Teaching bilingual learners: Effects of a language?based reading intervention on academic language and reading comprehension in grades 4 and 5. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(1), 95-122.

Pyle, N., Vasquez, A. C., Lignugaris/Kraft, B., Gillam, S. L., Reutzel, D. R., Olszewski, A., . . . Pyle, D. (2017). Effects of expository text structure interventions on comprehension: A meta?analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(4), 469-501. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.179

RAND Reading Study Group (2002). Reading for understanding, toward an R&D Program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.

Recht, D. R., & Leslie, L. (1988). Effect of prior knowledge on good and poor readers' memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(1), 16-20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.1.16

Relyea, J. E., Kim, J. S., Rich, P., & Fitzgerald, J. (2024). Effects of tier 1 content literacy intervention on early-grade English learners’ reading and writing: Exploring the mediating roles of domain-specific vocabulary and oral language proficiency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 116(7), 1172. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000882

Rice, M., & Wijekumar, K. (. (2024). Inference skills for reading: A meta-analysis of instructional practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 116(4), 569-589. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000855

Rice, M., Wijekumar, K., Lambright, K., & Bristow, A. (2024). Inferencing in reading comprehension: Examining variations in definition, instruction, and assessment.Technology, Knowledge and Learning: Learning Mathematics, Science and the Arts in the Context of Digital Technologies, 29(3), 1169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-023-09660-y

Rozen, S.D. (2005). Sentence disambiguation using syntactic awareness as a reading comprehension strategy for high school students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University.

Sabag-Shushan, T., Katzir, T., & Kanat-Maymon, Y. (2024). The contribution of emotion vocabulary to the reading comprehension of the text and the task. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-024-10609-5

Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11(1), 1-22.

Schmitt, N., Jiang, X., & Grabe, W. (2011). The percentage of words known in a text and reading comprehension. Modern Language Journal95(1), 26–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2011.01146.x 

Schmitz, A., Gräsel, C., & Rothstein, B. (2017). Students’ genre expectations and the effects of text cohesion on reading comprehension. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(5), 1115-1135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9714-0

Scott, C. M., & Balthazar, C. H. (2010). The grammar of information: Challenges for older students with language impairments. Topics in Language Disorders, 30(4), 288-307.

Scott, C.M., & Balthazar, C. (2013). The role of complex sentence knowledge in children with reading and writing difficulties. Perspectives on Literacy and Language, 39(3), 18-30.

Shanahan, T. (2025). Leveled reading, leveled lives. Cambridge: Harvard Education Press.

Sheehan, K. M. (2013). Measuring cohesion: An approach that accounts for differences in the degree of integration challenge presented by different types of sentences. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice, 32(4), 28-37. https://doi.org/10.1111/emip.12017

Shiotsu, T., & Weir, C. J. (2007). The relative significance of syntactic knowledge and vocabulary breadth in the prediction of reading comprehension test performance. Language Testing, 24

Comments

See what others have to say about this topic.

Rob Mar 29, 2025 02:07 PM

We need a clear example from you of a grade 4 lesson that demonstrates teacher practice that helps students transfer the skills/strategy for comprehension. The Gr 4 class has 23 students, 6 sped (reading at gr 3 level) and 3 EL students (reading at Gr 2 level).

Kayla Mar 29, 2025 02:12 PM

Tim - Thank you so much for helping to unpack this research! As a teacher of teachers, I am always looking at what the most important comprehension strategies are for me to pass along for use in classrooms where there never seems to be enough time.

Recently I have been trying to help my teachers understand the power of thinking aloud and practicing metacognition during reading. It seems to go along with your "instruction should help transform those into something comprehensible, without the teacher reading the books to the kids or telling them what they say."

I have a feeling that I will be reviewing and thinking about this analysis for a while!

Patrick Manyak Mar 29, 2025 03:36 PM

Tim, thanks for this cogent blueprint for effective comprehension instruction. One element that you allude to (and I know that you have focused on in many other places) but that I believe deserves a more specific shout out is written response to text. Graham and colleagues found that the mean effective size for having students write in response to a text that they read was larger than that of actually teaching comprehension strategies. And, in many of the studies, students engaged in "uninstructed response," i.e., they were simply prompted to write something about what they read. I don't think that it is a stretch to envision that "instructed response," teaching students to write thoughtful analytic/interpretive responses to texts may even have a still more robust effect. I like the guidance provided by the research of Matsumura, Correnti, & Wang in this area, and we directed our attention to this component in small-scale but very successful projects (Manyak, P., & Manyak, A.M. (2021). Literary analysis and writing: An integrated instructional routine. The Reading Teacher, 74, 395-405. doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1959) with literary texts and then later with informational texts (Manyak & Manyak (unpublished). Textual Analysis and Writing: A Routine for Informational Texts.) Our goal in these projects was to have students consistently write concise analytic responses to texts that crystallized prior oral discussion of key text concepts or structures while simultaneously teaching some specific tools that would improve the quality of their text-based writing. When I went back to classroom teaching, this kind of "textual analysis and writing" was very central to my 5th grade instruction, and students flourished in an empirically demonstrable way. Based on these experiences, I would personally add #7 - Consistent Focused Written Analysis of Texts...

Timothy Shanahan Mar 29, 2025 04:05 PM

Patrick--

I couldn't agree with you more. Thanks for this.

tim

Mary Baker-Hendy Mar 29, 2025 05:40 PM

Tim, I am in complete agreement on the negative effects of consistently selecting instructional level texts for students. Recommendation 4 of the IES Practice Guide, Providing Reading Intervention to Students 4-9th grade is to read “stretch text” with students receiving reading intervention.
But I am curious what you think about initially lowering the text demand when introducing literary elements concept such as theme or conflict/resolution. I have found that using a short story or essay to introduce a literacy element and then moving to grade level and higher text is a successful approach with students who are not practiced readers. Your thoughts?
Mary Baker-Hendy

Jamie Mar 29, 2025 05:45 PM

While I take on board what Capin is saying about questioning, I can't but think that there is still a place for some simple questioning about the test. I teach 1st grade, so perhaps that is relevant -- most of my students are new readers, and they do need to make the connection that they can answer simple questions and recall basic facts about what they are reading -- that they can read, themselves, and get actual information from those squiggles. It's not enough and 1st-graders can handle identifying who a pronoun refers to, etc. -- that seems to work best with a sort of hybrid of read-aloud/choral read in which I do the heavy decoding lift but keep their eyes on the text.

Shelley Bartolotti Mar 29, 2025 06:11 PM

I couldn't agree more with Durkin and Capin's findings regarding a lack of comprehension instruction. I find that in field of education, we often swing the pendulum so far to one side that begin ignoring the other significant components of reading. I am all for the initiative to get kids reading by using the science of reading and honing in on the critical factors such as phonemic awareness and phonics instruction but once this becomes proficient and even mastered, then what are we doing if not focusing on comprehension? Isn't the ultimate goal of reading to create meaning? We have gone so far as to buy into (literally) programs that are strong in building the foundational skills but then mesh social studies and science together with an array of articles from just about everywhere including former state tests. This is not how we to teach reading! Learning to read and understand the reading doesn't happen through osmosis. Once a child learns to read, that doesn't mean that the child is ready to take on comprehension on his/her own/. Comprehension needs explicit instruction as well as learning the code does. This is where we are lacking and I can speak as a reading teacher who has seen this dissolve over the last several years. The irony is that it has pretty much dissipated with the onset of the "science of reading" because we have all been forced to believe that our children can't read and therefore we have a crisis and getting them to read is all that matters. We threw the baby out with the bathwater when it comes to comprehension. Learning to read is essential and should be done in a systematic, explicit way that aims for mastery based on the science. But what about the other strands of Scarborough's rope? Do schools districts emphasize that there are many reading skills that must be woven together across the two broad areas of word recognition and language comprehension? Or do they mention it and only focus on certain parts? I don't think we can say we are taking structured literacy approach when we appear lopsided.

Timothy Shanahan Mar 29, 2025 06:12 PM

Mary-
What I've come to believe is that it is important to VARY the difficulty level. Every text should not be a big challenge, nor should every text be relatively easy and straightforward. There is no research directly on this issue, but I tried to suggest a difficulty regimen in my forthcoming book.

tim

Mary Baker-Hendy Mar 29, 2025 06:18 PM

Thanks Tim, that totally makes sense and is helpful advice to new teachers. I will look forward to reading the “difficulty regimen” in your new book. Mary BH

Rosalie Fleming Mar 29, 2025 08:21 PM

Reading this post and swiping past all the "proof" made me feel rather hopeless! Are you able to provide me with a unit of the type of comprehension lesson that would be ideal...and the amount of time it took you to plan it? I'm not a robot just a teacher.

Mat Mar 29, 2025 10:46 PM

Thank you Tim for this blog post. Wow, I counted 163 references- was that deliberate to show the sheer depth of evidence behind these aspects of reading comprehension?

Often your blog posts serve as good reminders to me of good practice- is the point about question routines something new that you have co side red or is this something you have mentioned in previous blogs. I’m curious if your thinking has changed based on recent reading.

Mirroring Rob snd Rosalie’s comments, would you consider a part 3 to this which provides more examples of these 6 ( now 7?) in a particular reading comprehension lesson. That would really helpful.

Many thanks!

Mary Mar 31, 2025 02:10 PM

I particularly appreciate your comments on executive functioning: "Reading comprehension like any cognitive activity falls under the control of this executive function.

There are many ways that instruction can address this: teaching students to try to comprehend (increasing intentionality) and how to comprehend (how to intentionally take comprehension supportive actions), and how to monitor success (recognizing when they’re comprehending and when they’re not, and how to fix that)."

I have been struggling to find help in how to teach these "ways." Foremost, my colleagues and administration don't even seem to share this concern (or don't ever articulate it) because, presumably, they rely on traditional approaches and as you say, "despite the research they don’t want to be convinced otherwise." Additionally, I teach high school English classes, from CP English for at risk students to AP English Language and Composition. Across the board, I see gaps in comprehension skills, and most resources address early grades and basic literacy. Can you share some resources that provide concrete strategies on how to teach those three primary skills you identified?

What Are your thoughts?

Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!

Comment *
Name*
Email*
Website
Comments

Are We Teaching Reading Comprehension? Part II – 6 Things Every Teacher Should Know

12 comments

One of the world’s premier literacy educators.

He studies reading and writing across all ages and abilities. Feel free to contact him.

Timothy Shanahan is one of the world’s premier literacy educators. He studies the teaching of reading and writing across all ages and abilities. He was inducted to the Reading Hall of Fame in 2007, and is a former first-grade teacher.  Read more

60 E Monroe St #6001
CHICAGO, Illinois 60603-2760
Contact Us Subscribe