Blast from the Past: This entry first posted on September 10, 2017, and reposted April 12, 2025. Blasts from the Past make blogs available to new audiences and afford me the opportunity to reiterate points or to reconsider my earlier claims. This site is dedicated to improving reading instruction through a close consideration of research. But research is an ongoing enterprise. Anyone who takes a principled stand based on science had better be prepared to dine on crow occasionally. This is one of those times. Accordingly, I have not rewritten this entry but hope to correct it with new prefatory and end notes. Perhaps comparing my current views with those originally expressed will illustrate what the impact of science should be.
A good deal of research on the value of morphology instruction has appeared since this blog first posted. These newer studies have strengthened the case for a larger and earlier place – and a broader purpose – for morphology instruction. As this blog pointed out, most research on morphology teaching showed that it increased vocabulary knowledge and improved the comprehension of older students. That is still true, and yet, increasingly studies reveal an impact on decoding and spelling with younger students and struggling readers. I still advocate for the primacy of phonics instruction in getting kids into the game, and that word knowledge should only take up a quarter of the instructional attention. Phonics and morphology though different, both contribute to the abilities to decode from print to pronunciation and spelling. Phonics is a simpler place to start, but morphology can begin to contribute early on. Peter and Jeffrey Bowers have been wise to argue for the early and ongoing inclusion of morphology in word study and for its ability to improve decoding – though possibly at times they have overstated these claims. In any event, I wish I’d paid more attention to it when I taught first grade.
The original blog entry included no references, this one is chock full! If you want to know what changed my mind, read this literature.
Man, sometimes when you publish a blog entry you’d wish you stayed in bed.
You hope to write something that someone will find useful. But the responses might make you feel more like you’ve been dropped onto the set of Fox News or MSNBC.
Recently, I’ve been smacked upside the head by several readers upset with me for not proposing more, and more thorough, spelling instruction and morphology instruction focused on spelling with the aim of improving decoding skills.
Some of those arguments have been energetic.
Traditional phonics instruction emphasizes letters and sounds but ignores the morphological and etymological reasons for spellings, my critics have pointed out. Reading experts have long recognized the importance of the morphological aspects of word meanings, but there has been little pedagogy aimed at the morphological aspects of spelling.
I’ve been sent lots of linguistic evidence to convince me of the morphological nature of our spelling system—and most of that work cites Dick Venezky’s seminal contributions.
In the 1960s, when computers first allowed for the large-scale quantitative study of language, Dick revealed the surprising consistency inherent in the English spelling system. Contrary to what had long been believed—that English spelling was a confusing mess—Venezky argued that whatever was lost in ease of pronunciation, was more than regained in the consistency of meaning inherent in our spellings. Hence, the endings of dogs and cats may be pronounced differently: /z/ and /s/, but their identical spelling consistently and helpfully signals plurality.
I’m happy to see that Dick’s work continues to bear fruit in linguistics (he was one of my teachers and friends—he even helped me to design morphology-oriented spelling measures for my doctoral dissertation). But I think he’d be surprised to hear his work used as an argument against phonics instruction – he was a big phonics proponent (though he too could be pretty critical about the designs of some popular phonics programs).
Dick not only had expertise in linguistics but extensive knowledge of psychology and computer science. He knew that teaching kids to read was different than inputting a linguistic system into a computer. Despite the flaws and shallowness of many (most) phonics programs when it comes to features like morphological sophistication, phonics teaching gives students a clear learning benefit.
What Dick Venezky came to believe was that phonics instruction gave students “clues” to the English spelling system. Students then rely on those clues to figure out how the system works. Phonics instruction does not teach everything one would need to “decode” text – kids are not electronic computers, but it provides them with useful pointers and puts students into a mindset of trying to recognize patterns and understand the system.
That doesn’t mean he would—or that we should—reject the idea of introducing morphological explanations and “clues” earlier in the curriculum, only that we shouldn’t be so sure that it would improve things as much as morphology proponents seem to assume.
For example, one colleague pointed out that in some phonics programs, kids are taught to divide the syllables of “action” in the following manner: ac/tion. He argued that this was a bad choice because it obscures that the root word is “act.” That’s correct linguistically, but does it matter when you’re 7?
Initially, we hope to teach kids enough to allow them to come up with an approximate pronunciation of a word that is already in their mental lexicons (primary grade kids know 5,000-10,000 words). It is more likely they’ll come up with “action” by saying “ak/shun” then by saying “act/ion.”
Of course, if they don’t know the word action—don’t know what the word action means—then, breaking the word in the second way (emphasizing “act”) may just get them to the meaning no matter the pronunciation.
The issue here turns on what would be best for beginning readers… is it best to help them to figure out the meanings of unknown words or to help them to translate print to pronunciations of words already in their oral language? I think it is the latter, so I don’t mind delaying most morphological work until phonics is mastered (e.g., Words Their Way).
However, there are arguments that we should teach morphology earlier and even in place of phonics instruction (one critic wrote that the National Reading Panel findings were out of date since we now know morphological training to be more beneficial than phonics). Eeks!
I looked at these critics’ evidence (Bowers & Bowers, 2017 provides a nice summary of this work). Specifically, there are two studies of morphological training for young children. One especially weak study—impossible to tell if the outcomes were due to the training or to existing ability differences in the participants—claimed long-term benefits to preschool morphology training.
The second was an experimental study that examined the impact of 10 hours of morphology teaching: This one claimed to enhance reading performance by more than a grade level! Not surprisingly, the outcome measures used were tightly aligned to the training and there were other design weaknesses, too.
That’s the entire body of instructional research one could use to prescribe instruction for preschool and primary grade kids (and in both studies, everyone got lots of phonics instruction, too—not exactly proof of the inadequacy of phonics).
Again, I can’t really say these folks are wrong—we might be able to affect clear reading improvement by teaching the morphological aspects of spelling earlier and more thoroughly, instead of what we currently provide with phonics.
But I won’t be prescribing reading instruction based on a single 10-hour study.
The reason why I insist that we teach phonemic awareness, phonics, spelling, vocabulary (word meanings including morphology), oral reading fluency, reading comprehension strategies, and writing is because there are dozens, even hundreds, of studies done by different researchers, with different kinds of kids, with different variations on the instructional routines, but with a consistent and substantial learning payoff. Why trust 100 such studies on phonics—some carried out for as long as 3-years—over a single small study of 10 hours of morphology instruction? I think you can probably answer that one for yourself.
I hope researchers will continue to propose provocative hypotheses about learning, and that they’ll continue to evaluate these ideas rigorously under a broad array of instructional conditions. And, if they find something that consistently helps kids, then I hope we’ll adopt their ideas. Until then, I won’t be recommending morphology over phonics or other terrific but unproven ideas—no matter how intelligently, reasonably, or vociferously those opinions may be stated.
Note posted April 12, 2025: Although this blog opposed much early attention to morphology, my views have changed as new evidence has accumulated. Most of this research has focused somewhat later in the primary grade sequence, so I still contend that it makes sense to start by teaching students to translate print to pronunciation (some form of phonics). However, I am increasingly being convinced that the early introduction of morphology make sense. Morphology teaching should be introduced even when explicit phonics instruction is still being taught. Gradually the proportion of word reading instruction should shift from phonology to morphology. The judicious approach would be to include some simple morphology lessons as early as kindergarten, increasing them as the new vocabulary (and spelling demands) justifies. What we need now are evaluations of experimental morphology curricula to help us understand how best to accomplish this.
References
Bear, D., Invernizzi, M., & Templeton, S. (2023). Words their way (7th ed). New York: Pearson.
Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 144-179. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654309359353
Colenbrander, D., Parsons, L., Bowers, J. S., & Davis, C. J. (2022). Assessing the effectiveness of structured word inquiry for students in grades 3 and 5 with reading and spelling difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Reading Research Quarterly, 57(1), 307-352. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.399
Crosson, A. C., Kieffer, M. J., McKeown, M. G., & Nagy, W. (2025). Cross-language morphological analysis improves academic word learning for multilingual adolescents. Scientific Studies of Reading, 29(1), 55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2024.2415916
Deacon, S. H., & Levesque, K. (2024). Mechanisms in the relation between morphological awareness and the development of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 116(6), 1052-1069. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000871
Fallon, K. A., & Katz, L. A. (2020). Structured literacy intervention for students with dyslexia: Focus on growing morphological skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(2), 336-344. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-19-00019
Gellert, A. S., Arnbak, E., Wischmann, S., & Elbro, C. (2021). Morphological intervention for students with limited vocabulary knowledge: Short? and long?term transfer effects. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(3), 583-601. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.325
Georgiou, G. K., Savage, R., Dunn, K., Bowers, P., & Parrila, R. (2021). Examining the effects of structured word inquiry on the reading and spelling skills of persistently poor grade 3 readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 44(1), 131-153. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12325
Goodwin, A. P., Petscher, Y., & Reynolds, D. (2022). Unraveling adolescent language & reading comprehension: The monster’s data. Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(4), 305-326. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.1989437
Gray, S. H., Ehri, L. C., & Locke, J. L. (2018). Morpho-phonemic analysis boosts word reading for adult struggling readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(1), 75-98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9774-9
Henbest, V. S., & Apel, K. (2017). Effective word reading instruction: What does the evidence tell us? Communication Disorders Quarterly, 39(1), 303-311. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740116685183
Kearns, D. M., & Al Ghanem, R. (2019). The role of semantic information in children’s word reading: Does meaning affect readers’ ability to say polysyllabic words aloud? Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(6), 933-956. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000316
Kronberg, N., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Lehecka, T., Vataja, P., & Salmi, P. (2024). Computer-assisted morphology training of reading with grade 2 and grade 3 poor readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-024-10605-9
Lee, J. w., Wolters, A., & Grace Kim, Y. (2023). The relations of morphological awareness with language and literacy skills vary depending on orthographic depth and nature of morphological awareness. Review of Educational Research, 93(4), 528-558. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543221123816
Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). Morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Examining mediating factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.015
Lyster, S. H., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Lervåg, A. O. (2021). Preschool phonological, morphological and semantic skills explain it all: Following reading development through a 9?year period. Journal of Research in Reading, 44(1), 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12312
McCutchen, D., Northey, M., Herrera, B. L., & Clark, T. (2022). What’s in a word? effects of morphologically rich vocabulary instruction on writing outcomes among elementary students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 35(2), 325-351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10184-z
Murphy, K. A., & Diehm, E. A. (2020). Collecting words: A clinical example of a morphology-focused orthographic intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(3), 544-560. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_LSHSS-19-00050
Park, Y., Brownell, M. T., Reed, D. K., Tibi, S., & Lombardino, L. J. (2020). Exploring how initial response to instruction predicts morphology outcomes among students with decoding difficulties. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(3), 655-670. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_LSHSS-19-00097
Saiegh?Haddad, E., & Taha, H. (2017). The role of morphological and phonological awareness in the early development of word spelling and reading in typically developing and disabled arabic readers. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 23(4), 345-371. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1572
Silverman, R. D., Johnson, E., Keane, K., & Khanna, S. (2020). Beyond decoding: A meta?analysis of the effects of language comprehension interventions on K–5 students’ language and literacy outcomes. Reading Research Quarterly, 55, S207-S233. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.346
Traga Philippakos, Z. A., Quinn, M. F., & Rocconi, L. M. (2024). Developing multisyllabic decoding and encoding skills with upper elementary learners: Reporting two cycles of design-based research. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2024.2406005
Venezky, R. (1967). English orthography: Its graphical structure and its relation to sound. Reading Research Quarterly, 2, 75–105. https://doi.org/10.2307/747031
Venezky, R. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.
Venezky, R. (1999). The American way of spelling. New York: Guilford.
Zhang, J., Zhang, H., Relyea, J. E., Wui, M. G. L., Yan, Y., Nam, R., . . . Kharabi-Yamato, L. (2023). Orthographic facilitation in upper elementary students: Does attention to morphology of complex words enhance the effects? Annals of Dyslexia, 73(1), 148-163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-022-00270-4
LISTEN TO MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Podcast
We resequenced HF word list for kinder so kids could see how the system works. We did emphasize phonics: ex: SAY/WAY/DAY/PLAY but then: PLAYS/PLAYED/PLAYING. In early reading days, kids with less experience tend to see every word as "100% new." We wanted them to see variations on base words in most accessible, productive way to show phonics and meaning chunks. We honored difficulty earlier: AT/AND/ASK and GET/YET/LET/LEFT so kids could experience blends: END/TEN/NET/WENT was good sequence: minimal contrasts = had to think. Fun, engaging language development (which our population greatly needed). "UP": sequence: UP/CUP/CUPS to demo not always a base word and plural. (We used the motif and physicality of "On Guard!" to signify pause and think, they could signal time to think.) Because UP is robust for reading, spelling, and basic, simple meaning for Ks we exploited it for language development: not just opposite of down but update, upside, etc., etc plus lots of interesting phrases, multiple meanings: up against, pull up, up the road, uptown, stand up guy, etc.
And of course root sequences like: GO/GOING/GOES/GONE. Ditto with DO. And these were in close sequence due to shared pattern. The kids picked out the pattern themselves. Also explicated meaning links: THEY, THEM, THEIR, THEY'RE. And the less difficult HERE, THERE, WHERE. We separated these "families" well in sequence so one is established first, then explicit contrast later made. Highly productive. We had to teach to requirements so just strengthened how we taught the words. So much potential as Tim is considering. Kids can do this if you lay the workings bare (and the weird things like OF... dubbed the strangest word). Engaged and guided kids to make connections to understand the system. And as research shows, strugglers can greatly benefit from explicit, elaborated teaching. You can see the wheels turning as they think. Give them time to process, this is not rote teaching, opposite of chants, etc.
As always, thank you for your post Dr. Shanahan. This point made me think of the multilingual learners I work with:
"Of course, if they don’t know the word action—don’t know what the word action means—then, breaking the word in the second way (emphasizing “act”) may just get them to the meaning no matter the pronunciation."
Do you think it makes sense to emphasize morphology more during decoding instruction for students who are learning to read in English and actually don't have many of these words in their oral language?
I work with struggling readers, and in first and second grade these students are easily confused. I don't go beyond inflections for these students. In third grade, I begin affixes. It really isn't until fourth grade that derivations and deeper word study seem developmentally appropriate. At this age, they seem to be able to grasp word analysis and use it to improve their reading. I also find that with young struggling readers, I have to keep things very clear and systematic with lots of repetition. I just can't see throwing derivations, affixes, and word analysis into the mix for them. Higher achieving readers could be a different story...
Thank you for revisiting such an important topic. I wrote about it a few months ago in "Must Phonics Fail in Order for Structured Word Inquiry to Succeed" (https://learningbydesign.com/professional-development/spell-links-blogue/). I think David Share says it best in his new article, "Blueprint for a universal theory of learning to read: The Combinatorial Model":
"Beginning readers of English are often (and wisely) first taught an oversimplified deterministic “rule” that the letter a (as in cat) makes the sound / æ/. Consider this as the first shoot at the single-letter layer or branch. This bit of information is not discarded as the reader grows, but is gradually refined into a more nuanced context-conditioned probabilistic (primarily implicit) understanding that this correspondence works well as a general default decoding (especially in short monosyllabic words such as cat, bag, and stand), but has other sounds in more complex words depending on position and context (take, call care, wand, farm, team, play, boat, etc.). . .
As print experience accumulates and decoding becomes more efficient, a growing (and interconnected/arborized) orthographic lexicon (or “sight vocabulary”) provides the resources for an expanding influence of word-level morphological and lexical factors . . . For example, English-speaking readers in the second and third grades read suffixed words such as hilly faster than matched pseudosuffixed words such as silly. It is widely agreed that this occurs only after the “basics” of decoding at the level of single letters and (sub-morphemic) combinations of letters such as digraphs have been mastered (as outlined in the previous section). However, sensitivity to some super-high frequency morphemes such as the English plural -s suffix and the past tense -ed which each have multiple phoneme values depending on the previous morpheme is surely acquired near the outset of reading."
Of course, that doesn't mean we can't introduce morphology well-before children have mastered phonics. I showed my kindergartners how to write 'ing' because they were writing stories about playing, running, kicking, etc.
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2025 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.