Blast from the Past: Is Morphology Training Better Than Phonics Instruction?

  • morphology phonics
  • 12 April, 2025
  • 14 Comments

Blast from the Past: This entry first posted on September 10, 2017, and reposted April 12, 2025. Blasts from the Past make blogs available to new audiences and afford me the opportunity to reiterate points or to reconsider my earlier claims. This site is dedicated to improving reading instruction through a close consideration of research. But research is an ongoing enterprise. Anyone who takes a principled stand based on science had better be prepared to dine on crow occasionally. This is one of those times. Accordingly, I have not rewritten this entry but hope to correct it with new prefatory and end notes. Perhaps comparing my current views with those originally expressed will illustrate what the impact of science should be.

A good deal of research on the value of morphology instruction has appeared since this blog first posted. These newer studies have strengthened the case for a larger and earlier place – and a broader purpose – for morphology instruction. As this blog pointed out, most research on morphology teaching showed that it increased vocabulary knowledge and improved the comprehension of older students. That is still true, and yet, increasingly studies reveal an impact on decoding and spelling with younger students and struggling readers. I still advocate for the primacy of phonics instruction in getting kids into the game, and that word knowledge should only take up a quarter of the instructional attention. Phonics and morphology though different, both contribute to the abilities to decode from print to pronunciation and spelling. Phonics is a simpler place to start, but morphology can begin to contribute early on. Peter and Jeffrey Bowers have been wise to argue for the early and ongoing inclusion of morphology in word study and for its ability to improve decoding – though possibly at times they have overstated these claims. In any event, I wish I’d paid more attention to it when I taught first grade.

The original blog entry included no references, this one is chock full! If you want to know what changed my mind, read this literature.

Man, sometimes when you publish a blog entry you’d wish you stayed in bed.

You hope to write something that someone will find useful. But the responses might make you feel more like you’ve been dropped onto the set of Fox News or MSNBC.

Recently, I’ve been smacked upside the head by several readers upset with me for not proposing more, and more thorough, spelling instruction and morphology instruction focused on spelling with the aim of improving decoding skills.

Some of those arguments have been energetic.

Traditional phonics instruction emphasizes letters and sounds but ignores the morphological and etymological reasons for spellings, my critics have pointed out. Reading experts have long recognized the importance of the morphological aspects of word meanings, but there has been little pedagogy aimed at the morphological aspects of spelling.

I’ve been sent lots of linguistic evidence to convince me of the morphological nature of our spelling system—and most of that work cites Dick Venezky’s seminal contributions.

In the 1960s, when computers first allowed for the large-scale quantitative study of language, Dick revealed the surprising consistency inherent in the English spelling system. Contrary to what had long been believed—that English spelling was a confusing mess—Venezky argued that whatever was lost in ease of pronunciation, was more than regained in the consistency of meaning inherent in our spellings. Hence, the endings of dogs and cats may be pronounced differently: /z/ and /s/, but their identical spelling consistently and helpfully signals plurality.

I’m happy to see that Dick’s work continues to bear fruit in linguistics (he was one of my teachers and friends—he even helped me to design morphology-oriented spelling measures for my doctoral dissertation). But I think he’d be surprised to hear his work used as an argument against phonics instruction – he was a big phonics proponent (though he too could be pretty critical about the designs of some popular phonics programs).

Dick not only had expertise in linguistics but extensive knowledge of psychology and computer science. He knew that teaching kids to read was different than inputting a linguistic system into a computer. Despite the flaws and shallowness of many (most) phonics programs when it comes to features like morphological sophistication, phonics teaching gives students a clear learning benefit.

What Dick Venezky came to believe was that phonics instruction gave students “clues” to the English spelling system. Students then rely on those clues to figure out how the system works. Phonics instruction does not teach everything one would need to “decode” text – kids are not electronic computers, but it provides them with useful pointers and puts students into a mindset of trying to recognize patterns and understand the system.

That doesn’t mean he would—or that we should—reject the idea of introducing morphological explanations and “clues” earlier in the curriculum, only that we shouldn’t be so sure that it would improve things as much as morphology proponents seem to assume.

For example, one colleague pointed out that in some phonics programs, kids are taught to divide the syllables of “action” in the following manner: ac/tion. He argued that this was a bad choice because it obscures that the root word is “act.” That’s correct linguistically, but does it matter when you’re 7?

Initially, we hope to teach kids enough to allow them to come up with an approximate pronunciation of a word that is already in their mental lexicons (primary grade kids know 5,000-10,000 words). It is more likely they’ll come up with “action” by saying “ak/shun” then by saying “act/ion.”

Of course, if they don’t know the word action—don’t know what the word action means—then, breaking the word in the second way (emphasizing “act”) may just get them to the meaning no matter the pronunciation.

The issue here turns on what would be best for beginning readers… is it best to help them to figure out the meanings of unknown words or to help them to translate print to pronunciations of words already in their oral language? I think it is the latter, so I don’t mind delaying most morphological work until phonics is mastered (e.g., Words Their Way).

However, there are arguments that we should teach morphology earlier and even in place of phonics instruction (one critic wrote that the National Reading Panel findings were out of date since we now know morphological training to be more beneficial than phonics). Eeks!

I looked at these critics’ evidence (Bowers & Bowers, 2017 provides a nice summary of this work). Specifically, there are two studies of morphological training for young children. One especially weak study—impossible to tell if the outcomes were due to the training or to existing ability differences in the participants—claimed long-term benefits to preschool morphology training.

The second was an experimental study that examined the impact of 10 hours of morphology teaching: This one claimed to enhance reading performance by more than a grade level! Not surprisingly, the outcome measures used were tightly aligned to the training and there were other design weaknesses, too.

That’s the entire body of instructional research one could use to prescribe instruction for preschool and primary grade kids (and in both studies, everyone got lots of phonics instruction, too—not exactly proof of the inadequacy of phonics).

Again, I can’t really say these folks are wrong—we might be able to affect clear reading improvement by teaching the morphological aspects of spelling earlier and more thoroughly, instead of what we currently provide with phonics.

But I won’t be prescribing reading instruction based on a single 10-hour study.

The reason why I insist that we teach phonemic awareness, phonics, spelling, vocabulary (word meanings including morphology), oral reading fluency, reading comprehension strategies, and writing is because there are dozens, even hundreds, of studies done by different researchers, with different kinds of kids, with different variations on the instructional routines, but with a consistent and substantial learning payoff. Why trust 100 such studies on phonics—some carried out for as long as 3-years—over a single small study of 10 hours of morphology instruction? I think you can probably answer that one for yourself.

I hope researchers will continue to propose provocative hypotheses about learning, and that they’ll continue to evaluate these ideas rigorously under a broad array of instructional conditions. And, if they find something that consistently helps kids, then I hope we’ll adopt their ideas. Until then, I won’t be recommending morphology over phonics or other terrific but unproven ideas—no matter how intelligently, reasonably, or vociferously those opinions may be stated. 

Note posted April 12, 2025: Although this blog opposed much early attention to morphology, my views have changed as new evidence has accumulated. Most of this research has focused somewhat later in the primary grade sequence, so I still contend that it makes sense to start by teaching students to translate print to pronunciation (some form of phonics). However, I am increasingly being convinced that the early introduction of morphology make sense. Morphology teaching should be introduced even when explicit phonics instruction is still being taught. Gradually the proportion of word reading instruction should shift from phonology to morphology. The judicious approach would be to include some simple morphology lessons as early as kindergarten, increasing them as the new vocabulary (and spelling demands) justifies. What we need now are evaluations of experimental morphology curricula to help us understand how best to accomplish this.

References

Bear, D., Invernizzi, M., & Templeton, S. (2023). Words their way (7th ed). New York: Pearson.

Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 144-179. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654309359353

Colenbrander, D., Parsons, L., Bowers, J. S., & Davis, C. J. (2022). Assessing the effectiveness of structured word inquiry for students in grades 3 and 5 with reading and spelling difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Reading Research Quarterly, 57(1), 307-352. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.399

Crosson, A. C., Kieffer, M. J., McKeown, M. G., & Nagy, W. (2025). Cross-language morphological analysis improves academic word learning for multilingual adolescents. Scientific Studies of Reading, 29(1), 55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2024.2415916

Deacon, S. H., & Levesque, K. (2024). Mechanisms in the relation between morphological awareness and the development of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 116(6), 1052-1069. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000871

Fallon, K. A., & Katz, L. A. (2020). Structured literacy intervention for students with dyslexia: Focus on growing morphological skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(2), 336-344. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-19-00019

Gellert, A. S., Arnbak, E., Wischmann, S., & Elbro, C. (2021). Morphological intervention for students with limited vocabulary knowledge: Short? and long?term transfer effects. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(3), 583-601. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.325

Georgiou, G. K., Savage, R., Dunn, K., Bowers, P., & Parrila, R. (2021). Examining the effects of structured word inquiry on the reading and spelling skills of persistently poor grade 3 readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 44(1), 131-153. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12325

Goodwin, A. P., Petscher, Y., & Reynolds, D. (2022). Unraveling adolescent language & reading comprehension: The monster’s data. Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(4), 305-326. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.1989437

Gray, S. H., Ehri, L. C., & Locke, J. L. (2018). Morpho-phonemic analysis boosts word reading for adult struggling readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(1), 75-98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9774-9

Henbest, V. S., & Apel, K. (2017). Effective word reading instruction: What does the evidence tell us? Communication Disorders Quarterly, 39(1), 303-311. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740116685183

Kearns, D. M., & Al Ghanem, R. (2019). The role of semantic information in children’s word reading: Does meaning affect readers’ ability to say polysyllabic words aloud? Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(6), 933-956. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000316

Kronberg, N., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Lehecka, T., Vataja, P., & Salmi, P. (2024). Computer-assisted morphology training of reading with grade 2 and grade 3 poor readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-024-10605-9

Lee, J. w., Wolters, A., & Grace Kim, Y. (2023). The relations of morphological awareness with language and literacy skills vary depending on orthographic depth and nature of morphological awareness. Review of Educational Research, 93(4), 528-558. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543221123816

Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). Morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Examining mediating factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.015

Lyster, S. H., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Lervåg, A. O. (2021). Preschool phonological, morphological and semantic skills explain it all: Following reading development through a 9?year period. Journal of Research in Reading, 44(1), 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12312

McCutchen, D., Northey, M., Herrera, B. L., & Clark, T. (2022). What’s in a word? effects of morphologically rich vocabulary instruction on writing outcomes among elementary students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 35(2), 325-351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10184-z

Murphy, K. A., & Diehm, E. A. (2020). Collecting words: A clinical example of a morphology-focused orthographic intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(3), 544-560. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_LSHSS-19-00050

Park, Y., Brownell, M. T., Reed, D. K., Tibi, S., & Lombardino, L. J. (2020). Exploring how initial response to instruction predicts morphology outcomes among students with decoding difficulties. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(3), 655-670. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_LSHSS-19-00097

Saiegh?Haddad, E., & Taha, H. (2017). The role of morphological and phonological awareness in the early development of word spelling and reading in typically developing and disabled arabic readers. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 23(4), 345-371. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1572

Silverman, R. D., Johnson, E., Keane, K., & Khanna, S. (2020). Beyond decoding: A meta?analysis of the effects of language comprehension interventions on K–5 students’ language and literacy outcomes. Reading Research Quarterly, 55, S207-S233. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.346

Traga Philippakos, Z. A., Quinn, M. F., & Rocconi, L. M. (2024). Developing multisyllabic decoding and encoding skills with upper elementary learners: Reporting two cycles of design-based research. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2024.2406005

Venezky, R. (1967). English orthography: Its graphical structure and its relation to sound. Reading Research Quarterly, 2, 75–105. https://doi.org/10.2307/747031

Venezky, R. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.

Venezky, R. (1999). The American way of spelling. New York: Guilford.

Zhang, J., Zhang, H., Relyea, J. E., Wui, M. G. L., Yan, Y., Nam, R., . . . Kharabi-Yamato, L. (2023). Orthographic facilitation in upper elementary students: Does attention to morphology of complex words enhance the effects? Annals of Dyslexia, 73(1), 148-163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-022-00270-4

 

Comments on Earlier Version

 

LISTEN TO MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Podcast

Comments

See what others have to say about this topic.

Tory Callahan Apr 11, 2025 11:58 AM

We resequenced HF word list for kinder so kids could see how the system works. We did emphasize phonics: ex: SAY/WAY/DAY/PLAY but then: PLAYS/PLAYED/PLAYING. In early reading days, kids with less experience tend to see every word as "100% new." We wanted them to see variations on base words in most accessible, productive way to show phonics and meaning chunks. We honored difficulty earlier: AT/AND/ASK and GET/YET/LET/LEFT so kids could experience blends: END/TEN/NET/WENT was good sequence: minimal contrasts = had to think. Fun, engaging language development (which our population greatly needed). "UP": sequence: UP/CUP/CUPS to demo not always a base word and plural. (We used the motif and physicality of "On Guard!" to signify pause and think, they could signal time to think.) Because UP is robust for reading, spelling, and basic, simple meaning for Ks we exploited it for language development: not just opposite of down but update, upside, etc., etc plus lots of interesting phrases, multiple meanings: up against, pull up, up the road, uptown, stand up guy, etc.
And of course root sequences like: GO/GOING/GOES/GONE. Ditto with DO. And these were in close sequence due to shared pattern. The kids picked out the pattern themselves. Also explicated meaning links: THEY, THEM, THEIR, THEY'RE. And the less difficult HERE, THERE, WHERE. We separated these "families" well in sequence so one is established first, then explicit contrast later made. Highly productive. We had to teach to requirements so just strengthened how we taught the words. So much potential as Tim is considering. Kids can do this if you lay the workings bare (and the weird things like OF... dubbed the strangest word). Engaged and guided kids to make connections to understand the system. And as research shows, strugglers can greatly benefit from explicit, elaborated teaching. You can see the wheels turning as they think. Give them time to process, this is not rote teaching, opposite of chants, etc.

Tara Barth Apr 12, 2025 02:43 PM

As always, thank you for your post Dr. Shanahan. This point made me think of the multilingual learners I work with:

"Of course, if they don’t know the word action—don’t know what the word action means—then, breaking the word in the second way (emphasizing “act”) may just get them to the meaning no matter the pronunciation."

Do you think it makes sense to emphasize morphology more during decoding instruction for students who are learning to read in English and actually don't have many of these words in their oral language?

Lauren Apr 12, 2025 03:22 PM

I work with struggling readers, and in first and second grade these students are easily confused. I don't go beyond inflections for these students. In third grade, I begin affixes. It really isn't until fourth grade that derivations and deeper word study seem developmentally appropriate. At this age, they seem to be able to grasp word analysis and use it to improve their reading. I also find that with young struggling readers, I have to keep things very clear and systematic with lots of repetition. I just can't see throwing derivations, affixes, and word analysis into the mix for them. Higher achieving readers could be a different story...

Harriett Janetos Apr 12, 2025 03:29 PM

Thank you for revisiting such an important topic. I wrote about it a few months ago in "Must Phonics Fail in Order for Structured Word Inquiry to Succeed" (https://learningbydesign.com/professional-development/spell-links-blogue/). I think David Share says it best in his new article, "Blueprint for a universal theory of learning to read: The Combinatorial Model":

"Beginning readers of English are often (and wisely) first taught an oversimplified deterministic “rule” that the letter a (as in cat) makes the sound / æ/. Consider this as the first shoot at the single-letter layer or branch. This bit of information is not discarded as the reader grows, but is gradually refined into a more nuanced context-conditioned probabilistic (primarily implicit) understanding that this correspondence works well as a general default decoding (especially in short monosyllabic words such as cat, bag, and stand), but has other sounds in more complex words depending on position and context (take, call care, wand, farm, team, play, boat, etc.). . .

As print experience accumulates and decoding becomes more efficient, a growing (and interconnected/arborized) orthographic lexicon (or “sight vocabulary”) provides the resources for an expanding influence of word-level morphological and lexical factors . . . For example, English-speaking readers in the second and third grades read suffixed words such as hilly faster than matched pseudosuffixed words such as silly. It is widely agreed that this occurs only after the “basics” of decoding at the level of single letters and (sub-morphemic) combinations of letters such as digraphs have been mastered (as outlined in the previous section). However, sensitivity to some super-high frequency morphemes such as the English plural -s suffix and the past tense -ed which each have multiple phoneme values depending on the previous morpheme is surely acquired near the outset of reading."

Of course, that doesn't mean we can't introduce morphology well-before children have mastered phonics. I showed my kindergartners how to write 'ing' because they were writing stories about playing, running, kicking, etc.

Timothy Shanahan Apr 12, 2025 04:38 PM

Tara-
I think the research encourages two things here: (1) Earlier attention to morphology and (2) Greater emphasis on flexibility and conditionality in phonics training (kids need to think about alternative possibilities).

tim

Jo Anne Gross Apr 12, 2025 07:50 PM

Tension in the field!
This week I listened to a webinar with Robert Pondiscio bc he tweeted about older kids that bump into a decoding threshold.
As remediators we see this constantly.
To my astoundment, the PhD on the show said when you see “cat” in the word education.
Very troubling
Ed-u-ca-tion
No cat.
I remember Mark Seidenberg saying there are even more exceptions in morphemes than there are in Phonics.

Shabari Apr 13, 2025 05:07 AM

The question that this raises is that given how unstable many of the syllable divisions are, is it worth the time spent teaching these patterns? When I say unstable, I mean even educators can’t agree on how to break some words up into syllables as these are based more on how words are pronounced than their meaning. It seems logical, that after students can decode and encode common single syllable words through phonics instruction, then teaching morphology and etymology could the next step. Do you think we have enough evidence that syllable teaching is the way to go?

Timothy Shanahan Apr 13, 2025 11:51 AM

Shabari--

I do think there is enough research evidence to suggest the value of a limited amount of syllable instruction as long as it is sufficiently conditional (providing alternative possibilities). This link provides that evidence:
https://www.shanahanonliteracy.com/blog/on-eating-elephants-and-teaching-syllabication-2

tim

Miriam Giskin Apr 13, 2025 01:15 PM

Thank you so much for addressing this. It is vital to do both. If you want to see how one can accomplish this with very young, preschool aged children, who have not yet begun to read please investigate the wrok of Rebecca Loveless. She does some amazing work. One simple game "In The Family" asks kids to notice if a word can belong with the others. They do not even have to be able to read at all. The teacher can actually do the reading and name the letters. So for example the following words would be in a bag; rain, raincoat, raining, rains, rainy, ranier, raniest, rained, rainbow, grain, ran. The child pulls out one word which the teacher (or child if they are able) spells and reads. You can also start with the word rain already laid out establishing that as the base. As each word is pulled from the bag it is used in a sentence, spelled, and placed with the base or not. There is much valuable discussion as it is established that it must have the same base (rain) to be in the designated "family" area. The child is learning the letters as they are repeated and the conversation about what is carrying the meaning is invaluable. Think about the discussions surrounding rainiest and most especially grain and ran! I have played this game with children and it really sparks their interest. Pronunciation discussions come up easily; think about the conversation regarding the letter O that would occur with do, don't and does. It is great work that points kids in the right direction rather than misrepresenting our orthographic system as so may phonics programs do!

Miriam Trehearne Apr 13, 2025 09:36 PM

Miriam P. Trehearne
Thanks again Tim for addressing this
Important issue. it also came up this week at The World Literacy Conference in Oxford, in my session. Again you clarify what the research truly says while keeping in mind the quantity of research in this area focussing on the early years. The research does NOT indicate an either or approach!

Pete Bowers Apr 14, 2025 03:18 PM

Thank you for this post Tim.

I understand why you present it as “eating crow,” but I would argue that changing our minds publicly based on new evidence, or a new analysis of evidence, should be the stuff of celebration in any scientific community. A post like this from a prominent scientist sharing a shift from previous positions provides many with a renewed openness to consider their own thinking. While you signal shifting more to my thinking, it will be the differences we still have that will be most interesting to discuss.

I wanted to address two points about your previous post for your readers.

Not Before But Together

First, I wanted to address the framing of title. You do not make this claim yourself, but people frequently frame Structured Word Inquiry (SWI) (Bowers & Kirby, 2010) as if it claims morphology instruction should happen “before” or “instead” of phonics. This is simply not the case.

SWI seeks to teach how the writing system works from the beginning. That is only possible if we explicitly teach grapheme-phoneme correspondences and influences from morphology and etymology that make sense of those correspondences from the beginning. One can disagree with that approach, but I wanted to make sure your readers recognize that I know of nobody working with SWI who recommends morphological instruction before or instead of teaching about grapheme-phoneme correspondences.

Anyone interested can see this document, “Guiding Principles, Concepts & Practices of Structured Word Inquiry (SWI)” I put recently in part to address this common mistaken view of SWI: https://tinyurl.com/y58atmxb

The Question of Chunking of “action”

Secondly I think I may be able to address your questions about dividing the word “action” into syllables (ac / tion) vs morphemes (act + ion) more effectively than I could back when you published in 2017.

Maryanne Wolf (2018) says that our attention systems are “biological spotlights.” I find this idea that you can’t make any mental connections (learn) where you do not attend helpful on this point.

In the earlier post, you wrote:

“For example, one colleague pointed out that in some phonics programs, kids are taught to divide the syllables of “action” in the following manner: ac/tion. He argued that this was a bad choice because it obscures that the root word is “act.” That’s correct linguistically, but does it matter when you’re 7?”

Of course attending to the and and and linking those spellings to to the pronunciation /æk/ and /??n/ can help many recognize the word without ever thinking about the spelling of the word “act.”

But the danger I see is not really about how a learner looks at a specific word. Instead, my fear is the effect of instruction that results in a schema that has learners default to “chunking” words into syllables when trying to recognize a word during reading, or spell them when writing.

In English, syllabic structures are not constrained my morphology. So the habit of mind for thinking in “syllabic chunks” will regularly favour attention to non-meaning bearing units at the expense of attending to spelling-meaning correspondences in morphemes.

My own anecdotal experience of shifting this default within a week of seeing my first matrix in 2001 from Real Spelling may be illustrative. (See current on-line version of Real Spelling here: https://www.tbox2.com/)

For most of my teaching career I put up a “Question Wall of Fame” sign in a section of my classroom from the first day of class. It was way to tell kids I was encouraging them I was not just looking for "answers" but excellent "questions" as well. I worked to make "questioning" a central driver learning in our class.

A week after I saw my first matrix at a Real Spelling presentation in 2001, I was sitting in my grade 4 classroom and that “Question Wall of Fame” poster grabbed my attention.

For the first time ever, I thought, “No way! Asking a question is going on a “quest” for understanding!” For 9 years I had been working to motivate my students to “question” their world. I could not believe that I had never noticed the “quest” before.

It turned out, I was not alone. A tiny minority of educators I work with have noticed the “quest” in “question” or that a “disease” is a system that is not “at ease.” before I draw their attention to these ideas with word sums or a matrix.

I can see no other explanation for this than the habit of mind of thinking in syllables“di/sease” or “ques/tion” directs our biological spotlights to leave those spelling-meaning correspondences of morphology in the dark. I am very curious if this frame plays a role for those labeled as "low comprehenders." These are people who have average or above decoding, but below average comprehension.

The other new data for me since your first post is the whole concept of the universal combinatorial nature of oral and written language.

I will address the link between that and the “action” question in a follow-up comment…

But many thanks for prompting this on-going discussion.

Pete

Peter Bowers Apr 14, 2025 03:20 PM

This comment follows on my previous one.

I wanted to highlight the role of "combinatoriality" in this question of "chunking" the word "action."

A couple of months ago, I posted a video (20 min) with my colleague Marie Foley who is an expert at illustrating complex orthographic concepts. We attempted to unpack this concept of the combinatorial structure of language, how it is realized in English, and how the matrix and word sum provide “combinatorial guard rails for literacy instruction. You can see that video here:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ptZl41tS9DA

Share’s (2025) article is quite astonishing in its scope and implications. But one of the articles he repeatedly pointed to highlighted another piece of the morphology and phonology puzzle that strikes me as having potentially profound implications. The Now-or-Never Bottleneck paper by Christiansen and Chater, 2016) makes the following statements may have important implications for understanding literacy learning that has not yet been part of the discussion:

“… the language system engages in eager processing when creating chunks. Chunks must be built right away, or memory for the input will be obliterated by interference from subsequent material.” (Christiansen & Chater, 2016, p. 9)

“The acoustic signal [phones] is first chunked into higher-level sound units at the phonological level. To avoid interference between local sound-based units, such as phonemes or syllables, these are further recoded as rapidly as possible into higher-level units such as morphemes or words.”

If I'm reading this correctly, according to combinatoriality, attending to oral morphology increases our capacity to “hold on” to phonology. This may help explain findings from morphological meta-analyses with the younger and less able gain the most from morphological instruction and Goodwin and Ahn’s (2010, 2013) findings that phonological outcomes saw the greatest benefit from morphological instruction.

I recently presented a 60 min talk “The Matrix Matters Because Language Is Combinatorial” for the 9th Annual Conference of the Dyslexia Training Institute that goes into much more detail about what combinatoriality is, why this structure is necessary to navigate the limits of cognitive processing and the fit with the matrix and word sums. They just made this talk from the conference public. I have not pointed to it yet elsewhere yet. Perhaps your readers may find it useful.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=M-aSzJAtF4Y

The last 5 minutes hones in on the quotes above.

Apologies for the long responses.

Thanks for your work Tim.

Jenny Chew Apr 15, 2025 12:33 PM

From Pete’s first comment above: ‘SWI seeks to teach how the writing system works from the beginning. That is only possible if we explicitly teach grapheme-phoneme correspondences and influences from morphology and etymology that make sense of those correspondences from the beginning’.

I’m trying to consider that in relation to Miriam Giskin’s comment about the game based on the family of ‘rain’ words, as Pete himself drew my attention, in 2019, to an SWI video showing a similar activity using ‘rain’ as a base. He, like Miriam, commented on the fact that the children were not expected to read the words but that they were being introduced to letter-names. By my count, 15 letters feature in the words Miriam mentions – that’s a lot of letters for beginners, and it’s only letter-names, with no mention of grapheme-phoneme correspondences or their practical application in reading and spelling. To my way of thinking, therefore, no phonics is involved.

In his second comment above, Pete mentions ‘combinatoriality’, as propounded by David Share in his 2024 ‘blueprint’ article – great stuff indeed. The paragraphs quoted by Harriett from that article, however, suggest that Share might envisage beginners being introduced to combinatoriality at an ‘oversimplified’ letter-sound level as a first step and only then moving on to larger units. He does say that ‘morphemes such as the English plural -s suffix and the past tense -ed which each which each have multiple phoneme values depending on the previous morpheme is surely acquired near the outset of reading’, but those are very common and pretty straightforward – and ‘near the outset’ doesn’t mean ‘at the outset’.

TESSA DAFFERN Apr 15, 2025 11:48 PM

Thank you for this thoughtful updated blog!
Teaching morphology starting in the first year of school is what I have advocated for through my PhD and subsequent research on the topic of spelling. Extensive data that I have gathered over the years show how early morphological instruction can be done and that it can be highly impactful. Specifically, I have lots of data to show that morphological skills can develop concurrently with phonics in the early years of school, if students are taught morphology. I have data that shows which morphological subskills appear easier and which seem to be harder. There is a lot of data that I have not published on this topic, but some of my published pieces related to this include:

Daffern, T. (2024). Developing spelling skills. In N.M Mackenzie., & J. Scull (Eds), Understanding and supporting young writers from birth to 8. (Chapter 6). Oxfordshire: Routledge. ISBN: 10.4324/9781315561301-7

Daffern, T. (2024). Teaching writing conventions. In E. Rata (Ed.), Research Handbook on Curriculum and Education (Vol. Part II Knowledge structures and the curriculum, pp. 228-244): Edward Elgar.

Daffern, T., Hogg., K., Callaway, N., Wild, H., & Kelly, S. (2024). Supporting schools to implement an evidence-based and effective approach to teaching spelling. Learning Difficulties Australia, 56(3), 31-38.

Daffern, T. (2022). Empowering teachers with an evidence-based spelling pedagogy. Practical Literacy: The Early and Primary Years, 27(2), 14-18.

Daffern, T., & Sassu, A. (2020). Building morphological foundations. Practical Literacy: The Early and Primary Years, 25(3), 35-37

Daffern, T., Mackenzie, N., & Hemmings, B. (2015). The development of a spelling assessment tool informed by Triple Word Form Theory. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, 38(2), 72-82.

Daffern, T. (2021). Exploring Australian students’ spelling skills. Oxford Children’s Language Australia. Oxford University Press

Daffern, T. (2020). Assessment informed practices for teaching spelling in the early years: Early Action for Success (EAfS) Pathway. Report submitted to the NSW Department of Education

Also ... here are links to 2 videos involving 5-year old children, about 4 months into their first year of schooling (Foundation year):

- Foundations in Spelling 1: https://youtu.be/mic6O14ZI0w?si=piVwU60OZuLlvmte
- Foundations in Spelling 2 (a child reading a text that the students in the class co-wrote following the lessons shown in the previous video): https://youtu.be/mic6O14ZI0w?si=piVwU60OZuLlvmte

Plus: This is a bit of an old video now, but in it, I outline some information on the Components of Spelling Test for the Early Years (CoSTEY): https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=8dtXNYkfJVg&list=UU5pvqoUDTNouQSHcPFOXS-w&index=31&t=25s
The CoSTEY has 3 component tests (phonology, orthography and morphology - aligned with Triple Word Form Theory). Australian norm-references are included for each component.

Also: here is a recorded version of a webinar where I outline the resources that I have developed to support teachers to teach the components of spelling, including morphology, from the first year of school. https://youtu.be/XkTx05q2wBs?si=vLYO0JCeRUNvd4Vi
For example, I talk about the CoSTEY Dashboard - it contains examples of scope and sequences where morphology is included and aligned with the phonological and orthographic components of spelling. Morphological activities are tailored based on the error analysis from the CoSTEY.

Feel free to get in touch with me to chat further if you wish. My best email is tessa@daff.net.au

Thanks again for your valuable insights.

Dr Tessa Daffern

What Are your thoughts?

Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!

Comment *
Name*
Email*
Website
Comments

Blast from the Past: Is Morphology Training Better Than Phonics Instruction?

14 comments

One of the world’s premier literacy educators.

He studies reading and writing across all ages and abilities. Feel free to contact him.

Timothy Shanahan is one of the world’s premier literacy educators. He studies the teaching of reading and writing across all ages and abilities. He was inducted to the Reading Hall of Fame in 2007, and is a former first-grade teacher.  Read more

60 E Monroe St #6001
CHICAGO, Illinois 60603-2760
Subscribe