Can we really teach prosody and why would we want to?

  • prosody oral reading fluency
  • 08 July, 2023
  • 11 Comments

Teacher question:

How expressive should young children’s reading be? We are told that oral reading fluency consists of accuracy, rate, and prosody, but our monitoring tests only consider accuracy and rate. Does prosody matter in first and second grade and if it does how do we measure it?

RELATED: What is linguistic comprehension in the simple view of reading?

Shanahan responds:

Experts have long accepted the idea that oral reading fluency (ORF) or, these days, “text reading fluency”, improves simultaneously with reading development. As readers progress, they can read more words accurately, they are able to do this with less conscious effort (automaticity), and their prosody gets better, too.

The funny thing is that no one questions whether those accuracy or automaticity gains are central to reading development. The benefits of teaching kids to recognize words proficiently seems obvious to everyone.

But the role of prosody is not as well accepted.

Some experts view prosody as no more than a positive side effect. These experts believe that when students can read with comprehension, they’ll automatically make the text sound meaningful. Their advice to teachers?  Don’t sweat the prosody!

While other authorities believe prosody is just like accuracy and automaticity – if prosody doesn’t improve, then reading can’t get better either. According to this view, teachers should try to teach prosody.

My sense is that more and more, the evidence is coming down on the “prosody matters” side.

So, what is prosody?

The National Reading Panel described prosody rather straightforwardly as “proper expression.” I wrote that definition myself, but when I talk to teachers (and kids) I tend to offer an even more down to earth explanation… it just means “making the text sound like spoken language.” (Don’t take me too literally on that… when I read silently, I want the text in my head to sound like spoken language, too.)

For a more scholarly definition, I turn to Melanie Kuhn and her colleagues: Prosody is “appropriate expression or intonation coupled with phrasing that allows for maintenance of meaning” (Kuhn, Schwanenflugel, & Meisinger, 2010). This, they clarify, includes variations in frequency or pitch, duration of vowels, stress on syllables, and pausing. They believe that these prosodic features are used to break up or parse text into meaningful units, to manage the information included in the syntax of the sentences and the discourse properties of the text (Miller & Schwanenflugel, 2006, 2008).  

Given that definition, then text reading fluency is about both decoding and comprehending. That’s why, in the Active Model of Reading (Duke & Cartwright, 2021), fluency is included in the “Bridging Variables” category – the place for variables that are both fish and fowl.

The earliest conjectures on the value of fluency instruction focused on decoding alone: fluency practice increases sight vocabulary (Rashotte & Torgesen, 1985), enhances the ability to apply phonics skills (Chomsky, 1978), and supports automaticity development (Samuels, 1979). Each of these claims, hypothesizes fluency to be about better decoding, which in turn might enable better comprehension. Fluency practice, then, was not expected to have any direct impact on understanding.

Accordingly, it shouldn’t be surprising that most studies of fluency teaching have focused either on beginning readers or struggling readers (National Reading Panel, 2000), those students most likely to need attention to their decoding skills.

But more recent research shows that text reading fluency is more closely related to reading comprehension than is word reading fluency (Kim, 2015; Wise, et al., 2010). You wouldn’t expect to see that kind of difference if text fluency was nothing more than decoding. If word reading fluency and text reading fluency were the same, they’d have a similar relationship with comprehension. But that’s not the case. Something else is evidently going on in text reading that doesn’t happen in word reading, and it appears that this difference is bound up in the prosody part of fluency.

Recent research shows there to be a significant, though moderate relationship between prosody and comprehension (Wolters, Kim, & Szura, 2022). I don’t think we should expect more than that from prosody since other factors that play a role in comprehension, too.

Complicating matters here is the fact that prosody is a complex variable with multiple parts, and – at this point – with unclear connections among those parts. Scientists are still trying to figure out the best way to measure each of those parts.

In any event, the durations and frequencies of pausing, intonation, expressiveness, smoothness, and pitch are complicated things to measure, especially in the varying contexts that different texts present.

The reading of more difficult text leads to less prosodic reading and no one knows what reader-text match is most appropriate for estimating prosody properly. Perhaps the best estimate would come not from the reading of a single text or even from an average of multiple readings of texts at a given level. Maybe it would entail multiple readings of different levels of text or some kind of improvement score based upon rereading.

An important finding that Wolters and company (2022) reported was that, at this point, the prosody rating scales do better than the more technical spectrogram assessments that more precisely measure sound waves to the milliseconds. That’s good to know. It suggests that teachers can monitor prosody sufficiently by listening to students’ oral reading and rating its quality.

My favorite among the rating schemes?

For classroom use, I prefer the simplest one. I think teachers would be most able and willing to use the National Assessment’s scale (Daane, Campbell, Grigg, Goodman, & Oranje, 2005). I’ve used it myself in many schools and have found it to be satisfactory.

The NAEP measure one is a 4-point-scale and it focuses mainly on pausing, a variable with a clear relationship to reading comprehension. Rather than trying to quantify expressiveness (since our goal isn’t to get kids to deliver lines like Jack Nicholson or Meryl Streep), this approach emphasizes how the words are grouped by the students.  

If a text is read choppily word by word, it is rated a 1.

If the pauses usually come every second or third word but without attention to the grammar or punctuation, that reading gets a 2.

Both of those scores show disfluency that would be expected to undermine or interfere with comprehension.

Threes and 4s reflect pauses between multiword segments, but most importantly, these pauses reflect the punctuation and grammar of the sentences. The text so read will sound meaningful.

If you listen to that kind of reading, you can grasp the meaning by ear without having the text before you. It’s hard to do that when the pausing merits lower ratings.

Obviously, there is more to prosody than pausing. But pausing seems to be a powerful proxy for the whole thing. Kids who aren’t pausing in appropriate places probably aren’t doing much else to make the text meaningful.

Here’s a sentence that I have taken from a fourth-grade reading textbook: Good business is not always about the bottom line.

Here are four ways this text may be read. The slashing lines show the pauses, and the numbers indicate their ratings.

1.     Good // business // is not // always // about the // bottom // line.

2.     Good // business is // not always // about the // bottom// line.

3.     Good business // is not // always about the // bottom line.

4.     Good business // is not always about // the bottom line.

The differences may not seem especially great when considering only one 9-word sentence, but now imagine that you are reading (or listening to) an entire passage presented in those ways – along with the word reading errors, repetitions, and intonation problems that are also likely to accompany these pausing patterns.

You ask how expressive your students should be? My response is that their oral reading should suggest that they are understanding what they are reading. These renditions should be adequate to support a listener’s comprehension as well. If it doesn’t sound like the student understands the text, then it lacks sufficient prosody.

Teachers should listen for those kinds of weaknesses and should provide students with instruction aimed at helping them to do better.

One instructional approach found to improve the prosody of early readers is repeated reading (Logan, 1997; Stoddard, Valcante, Sindelar, O'Shea, & et al., 1993). Having students read texts aloud that they cannot already read well – and doing so two or three times to try to read it better can have positive effects on accuracy, automaticity, and prosody. One small study found that having the kids work on their expressiveness during repeated reading improved comprehension (Calet, Pérez-Morenilla, & De los Santos-Roig, 2019). Such rereading practice may include encouraging kids to make questions sound like questions or using their voice to allow a listener to distinguish narration from dialogue.

Another successful approach is to provide guidance in how to group words within sentences.

One of my favorite studies of all time administered standardized reading comprehension tests to students. The test passages were either parsed for the students – marking where the pauses should go – or they weren’t. The kids who had the parsed texts outscored the others by a full grade level (Stevens, 1981)! Knowing where the pauses went had a powerful impact on these students’ abilities to answer questions about the text.

Studies have shown – though usually with older students – that we can teach students to chunk sentences into their meaningful parts (O’Shea & Sindelar, 1983). Do guided practice with that, help students to figure out where to pause, and how to recognize prepositional phrases (and other kinds of phrases). As with other kinds of teaching, your lessen the guidance as students gain proficiency.

I would do such work with texts that the class would otherwise be working with – such as this week’s reading selection or the chapter in our social studies book.

I know there are materials aimed at giving kids practice reading phrases. I know of no research into the effectiveness of such practice, though as students gain reading proficiency, the amount of time between the words in high frequency phrases declines. In other words, those phrases stat to become identifiable as phrases – and this occurs as early as Grade 1. That could mean that such phrase reading practice deserves some attention.

Also, there has been a good deal of work into sentence comprehension, and I suspect that, too, would help with prosody development. Here is a link to a recent blog I wrote on that subject: https://www.shanahanonliteracy.com/blog/trying-again-what-teachers-need-to-know-about-sentence-comprehension

Remember, the point of all of this is to make the reading sound meaningful. If students can convert the words on the page into sentences that sound meaningful, they will then be on their way to fuller comprehension and better reading.

READ MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Blog

References

Calet, N., Pérez-Morenilla, M. C., & De los Santos-Roig, M. (2019). Overcoming reading comprehension difficulties through a prosodic reading intervention: A single-case study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 35(1), 75-88. doi.org/10.1177/0265659019826252

Chomsky, C. (1978). When you still can’t read in third grade: After decoding, what? In S. J. Samuels (Ed)., What research has to say about reading instruction (pp. 13-30). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Daane, M.C., Campbell, J.R., Grigg, W.S., Goodman, M.J., & Oranje, A. (2005). Fourth-grade students reading aloud: NAEP 2002 special study of oral reading (NCES 2006-469). U.S. Department of Education. Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

Duke, N.K., & Cartwright, K.B. (2021). The science of reading progresses: Communicating advances beyond the simple view of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(S1), 

S25–S44. doi.org/10.1002/rrq.411

Kim, Y. G. (2015). Developmental, component?based model of reading fluency: An investigation of predictors of word?reading fluency, text?reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 50(4), 459-481. doi.org/10.1002/rrq.107

Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(2), 230-251. doi:https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.2.4

Logan, G.D. (1997) Automaticity and Reading: Perspectives from the Instance Theory of Automatization. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 13, 123-147. doi.org/10.1080/1057356970130203

Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J.  (2006). Prosody of syntactically complex sentences in the oral reading of young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(4). doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.839

Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P.J. (2008). A longitudinal study of the development of reading prosody as a dimension of oral reading fluency in early elementary schoolchildren. Reading Research Quarterly, 43(4), 336–354. Doi: 10.1598/RRQ.43.4.2

National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: an evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

O’Shea, L. J., & Sindelar, P. T. (1983). The effects of segmenting written discourse on the reading comprehension of low- and high-performance readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 458–465.

Rashotte, C. A., & Torgesen, J. K. (1985). Repeated reading and reading fluency in learning disabled children. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(2), 180-188. doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.20.2.4

Samuels, S. J. (1979). The method of repeated reading. Reading Teacher, 32(4), 403-408.

Stevens, K. (1981). Chunking material as an aid to reading comprehension. Journal of Reading, 25, 126-129.

Stoddard, K., Valcante, G., Sindelar, P., O'Shea, L., & et al. (1993). Increasing reading rate and comprehension: The effects of repeated readings, sentence segmentation, and intonation training. Reading Research and Instruction, 32(4), 53-65.  doi.org/10.1080/19388079309558133

Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., Wolf, M., Kuhn, M., . . . Schwanenflugel, P. (2010). The relationship between different measures of oral reading fluency and reading comprehension in second-grade students who evidence different oral reading fluency difficulties. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41(3), 340-348. doi:https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0093)

Wolters, A., Kim, Y.G., & Szura, J.W. (2022). Is reading prosody related to reading comprehension? A meta-analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 26(1), 1-20.

Comments

See what others have to say about this topic.

Dr. Gwen Lavert Jul 08, 2023 01:57 PM

Students should be taught to read like they are speaking. I have found that so many students can’t read like they are speaking because they are still struggling with “automatic” decoding. However, there as so many strategies that can improve this; for example, poetry, Hip! Hop, and short plays.

Tara Jul 08, 2023 01:58 PM

Thank you so much for the post, I always appreciate your insight!

I have read that text-reading fluency doesn't predict comprehension for English learners in the same way it does for monolingual students, unless ELs have fairly strong oral language skills. Does this have any implications for the way we assess and instruct ELs in fluency/prosody?

Thanks,

Tara

Laura Jul 08, 2023 02:22 PM

From the child’s perspective, a prosodic reader is a joyful reader!
I love helping children to improve this artful component of fluency.
Thank you for your many important contributions, Mr. Shanahan.

Timothy Shanahan Jul 08, 2023 04:26 PM

Tara--

I would expect the payoffs from oral reading fluency to be somewhat lower for English Learners -- particularly in terms of the gains to be expected from the accuracy and automaticity (my work was the first to report a lower phonics payoff for English Learners). However, given the role that prosody plays, I would be surprised if that wasn't as effective or nearly as effective for them because of its language role. However, I know of no direct evidence on this.

tim

Ann Christensen Jul 08, 2023 05:08 PM

Children recognize and appreciate prosody when being read to. Naive readers do not automatically know they are to listen to themselves as they read. This lack of listening affects both comprehension and prosody. They are saying a word list. Kindergarteners can understand how to read to their own brain if explicitly taught. This increases phrasing, rate, and prosody.
I have also found this to be true of older, struggling readers.

Julie Jul 08, 2023 06:17 PM

I wonder if systematic and explicit teaching of the function of punctuation in writing would be helpful to developing prosody in reading. I suspect that teachers who just teach students to "put the commas where the pauses go", without developing an understanding of compound and complex sentences, appositives, dependent and independent clauses, restrictive and non-restrictive clauses, etc., are missing an opportunity to help students understand the structure of sentences. If so, how does that affect their students' ability to develop prosody and comprehension?

Timothy Shanahan Jul 08, 2023 10:43 PM

Julie--

I know of no studies of that, but I assume that you are correct... teaching kids about punctuation in their writing should contribute to their prosody development.

tim

Dr. Bill Conrad Jul 09, 2023 01:36 AM

Hip Hip Hooray for this awesome article about prosody! Read orally with a full expression of happiness and joy! Pause! Well done article as usual! In praise of prosody!

Dr Rosalind Horowitz Jul 09, 2023 02:40 PM

See Rosalind Horowitz in “Transforming speech into writing” in The Routledge International Handbook of Research on Writing (2023) This chapter addresses prosody as vital to creating a voice in written language. Prosody ias been neglected for too long in writing and the reading of writing across the grades.

William Keeney Jul 10, 2023 05:42 PM

I have been working with adolescent prosody for a long time now. The two questions I keep coming up against are: is prosody the cause of better comprehension, or is comprehension the cause of better prosody? If the latter, teaching prosody as a way to improve comprehension is not effective. More likely (in my experience) it is the re-readings that improve comprehension or, if, as in the study you presented about chunking the sentences, the phrasing is done by someone who already understands the passage, which gives them a leg up on the comprehension--not necessarily their own independent comprehension.

The second one is: does instruction in prosody actually improve comprehension as applied to novel texts? Some studies have shown that there is not much transfer to novel texts, and when it does it is almost always only in the subject at hand--as you noted in your example. So, the first question you raise is really the important one to me. CAN we teach prosody? Does it result in better comprehension--not for this particular piece, but for the next and the next? Or will the prosody follow on a whole host of independent, wide reading that the typically developing reader gets from about 3rd grade on?

The follow-up question is: IF we can promote prosody in our developing readers, what evidence-based techniques work and which are the best for which readers at which stage? [My guess is it isn't those online "fluency" programs, whose names you probably know well.)

My last comment--and I apologize for the length, but this is a question I have been pursuing passionately for a while--is: rather than teaching "prepositional phrases," which is a grammatical term but not one that has much importance for syntactical structure, is to teach Noun Phrases, Verb Phrases, Adjectival Phrases and Adverbial Phrases, as these are the ones that create the meaning of the sentence. [The boy under the table = which boy?; put his feet under the table = where did he put them?).

Timothy Shanahan Jul 10, 2023 05:51 PM

William--

The studies that I cited did show generalization to other texts. Teaching at least some students where to pause has been found to improve reading comprehension more generally during adolescence.

Prepositional phrases can be quite important, particularly in the reading of science text (in science, clausal structure tends to be less important than phrasal structure (science tends to string together multiple phrases in a manner less common in literature and history).

tim

What Are your thoughts?

Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!

Comment *
Name*
Email*
Website
Comments

Can we really teach prosody and why would we want to?

11 comments

One of the world’s premier literacy educators.

He studies reading and writing across all ages and abilities. Feel free to contact him.