The new common core standards are challenging widely accepted instructional practices. Probably no ox has been more impressively gored by the new standards than the widely-held claim that texts of a particular difficulty level have to be used for teaching if learning is going to happen.
Reading educators going back to the 1930s, including me, have championed the idea of there being an instructional level. That basically means that students would make the greatest learning gains if they are taught out of books that are at their “instructional” level – meaning that the text is neither so hard that the students can’t make sense of them or so easy that there is nothing in them left to learn.
These days the biggest proponents of that idea have been Irene Fountas and Gay Su Pinnell, at Ohio State. Their “guided reading” notion has been widely adopted by teachers across the country. The basic premises of guided reading include the idea that children learn to read by reading, that they benefit from some guidance and support from a teacher during this reading, and, most fundamentally, that this reading has to take place in texts that are “just right” in difficulty level. A major concern of the guided-readingistas has been the fear that “children are reading texts that are too difficult for them.”
That’s the basic idea, and then the different experts have proposed a plethora of methods for determining student reading levels, text difficulty levels, and for matching kids to books, and for guiding or scaffolding student learning. Schemes like Accelerated Reader, Read 180, informal reading inventories, leveled books, high readability textbooks, and most core or basal reading programs all adhere to these basic ideas, even though there are differences in how they go about it.
The common core is based upon a somewhat different set of premises. They don’t buy that there is an optimum student-text match that facilitates learning. Nor are they as hopeful that students will learn to read from reading (with the slightest assists from a guide), but believe that real learning comes from engagement with very challenging text and a lot of scaffolding. The common core discourages lots of out-of-level teaching, and the use of particularly high readability texts. In other words, it champions approaches to teaching that run counter to current practice.
How could the common core put forth such a radical plan that contradicts so much current practice?
The next few entries in this blog will consider why common core is taking this provocative approach and why that might be a very good thing for children’s learning.
Stay tuned.
7/5/2011
This statement surprises me greatly, "You’d be hard pressed these days to find teachers or principals who don’t know that literal recall questions that require a reader to find or remember what an author wrote are supposed to be harder than inferential questions (the ones that require readers to make judgments and recognize the implications of what the author wrote)."
I recently listed to a great podcast from Daniel T. Willingham about the importance of background knowledge (http://download.publicradio.org/podcast/americanradioworks/podcast/arw_4_30_reading.mp3?_kip_ipx=1017638692-1297712350) In that podcast Willingham shares this example:
"I just got a puppy and my landlord is not too happy."
If what you say is true, then readers should be able to more easily infer why the landlord is not happy than they would be able to answer this question, "What did I just get?" Hmmmm...... I am really hoping this was a typo and not that I am missing out on a lot of reading research. If it was not a typo, could you please let me know what research supports your statement?
7/5/2011
Julie-
No, it is not a typo. It is the text that matters, not the question type. You are certainly correct that you can construct experimental texts for which it may be relatively easier or harder to answer one or another question type. In your example, the inferential question was harder because most readers would get the explicitly stated idea, but they might differ in their background knowledge about landlords and puppies.
However, look at this text:
John put on his sunglasses as he explored the ambit of the range.
Which question is easier, one that asks why John put on his sunglasses (an easy inference) or one that asks what he did (it is explicitly stated, but since most people don't know the meaning of ambit, it is the harder question in this case)?
The best evidence that we have of this isn't these kinds of tortured comparisons with artificial texts, but how hundreds of thousands of readers perform across dozens of naturally occurring texts with hundreds of real questions. That research is cited and linked in the blog that you commented on. Pay special attention to the charts showing the differences between literal and inferential performance at lots of different comprehension levels.
thanks.
tim
7/5/2011
Thank you so much for the response, Dr. Shanahan. I look forward to reading the article you shared.
In the meantime,your example has me confused. I would say that the literal question is easier to answer. I could easily answer, "What did John do?" with, "John put on his sunglasses as he explored the ambit of the range." It does not necessarily mean I understand the sentence, but I can answer it correctly.
On the other hand, without some background knowledge of why people wear sunglasses and an understanding of "ambit" and "range," I could not answer your inferential question, "Why did John put on his sunglasses?" Hmmm...
Finally, I would say that the background knowledge of the reader is most important--without it, appropriate inferences cannot be made.
Again, thank you for your time.
7/5/2011
But see, Julie, that's where we get into trouble... when we start to say, "I could answer the comprehension question, I just couldn't understand what it meant." That means that the question required dumb pattern matching, without any real understanding of the author's message. That can't be acceptable.
Background knowledge certainly matters, but not just for inferences. How did you know what a puppy was or what it means to get one, or a landlord, or happiness? Interpreting those words requires prior knowledge, if only of the words themselves. That reading comprehension requires the combination of the new (what the author has told you) and the known (what you bring to the text), doesn't make one kind of question generally easier or harder than another.
tim
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2024 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.