Blast from the Past: This entry first posted in February 28, 2105 and it was reposted on July 31, 2021. The only change that I needed to make in this one was to add more research studies -- they continue to accumulate. This entry challenges both the idea of the instructional reading level and that resource teachers should be focused on that with any but beginning readers. I hope this helps you to rethink some practices that are holding kids back. Teacher question: Please provide the research about how teaching students using instructional level texts does not yield results! I am a literacy coach with five years of successful guided reading with below-level ELL's, working with them at their instructional level for TWENTY MINUTES A DAY. The rest of our two-hour block is spent with students immersed in either an independent book of their choice (also about 20-25 minutes) or in grade level text (1+ hours). I feel confident that I am teaching CCSS Standard 10 because my students read complex text in whole group with my scaffolding. I understand you've probably posted it many times, but please post it again here so I can see the research about why these 20 minutes of my students' day, where I see them growing by leaps and bounds, is actually preventing them from achieving the Common Core standards! Shanahan response: I’ve never written that no learning results from being taught from texts at one’s instructional level. In fact, the majority U.S. kids are currently taught in that fashion—and most American kids are learning to read, albeit not as well as we want them to. I have no doubt that your students are learning something from the instructional level teaching that you are offering them. But the real issue has to do with what’s best for kids, rather than what works. The men and women who manned the “iron lungs” of the 1950s did much for polio victims. No doubt about it. But they didn’t do as much as Sabin and Salk who took a different approach to the matter. Iron lungs worked. Polio vaccines worked better. Teaching kids at their instructional level works. But you can often do better if you give kids the opportunity to learn more by placing them in more challenging texts. You don’t indicate which grade level you teach, so it’s important to stress that instructional level appears to matter initially—that’s when kids are first learning to read—but it doesn’t seem to matter after that. Perhaps you are working with first-graders or kids who are reading at a first-grade level, in which case, I think you're going the right direction. (Of course, if you’re talking about kids who can read at a second- grade level and up, then I’d question why you are teaching everyone as if they were first-graders.) Your instructional use of time seems peculiar to me. Two hours of reading class with no explicit instruction in decoding, fluency, vocabulary, or comprehension? I know there are fans of the idea that we just learn to read by reading, and I’ve certainly been critical about the lack of reading within instruction, but the research records on explicit teaching of the skills noted above--including to English learners--are just too good to ignore. Teaching any of the skills listed above has several times the impact on kids’ reading growth than having them off reading on their own. (I do encourage kids to read independently when I don’t have a highly skilled teacher available to work with them, but having them off reading separately from instruction when I do have such a teacher available is wasteful.) Unlike what has been traditionally proposed by guided reading advocates, I have supported the idea of teaching kids with texts at multiple levels. That is, not all of the required reading should be at a student’s instructional level. Learning and consolidation come from taking on different levels of challenge—varying the workload from easy to strenuous. I like that you are intentionally having students read texts at multiple levels of demand. Nevertheless, I’m puzzled as to why you work so closely with children when you believe they will have little or no difficulty with a text (you indicate that you work in small groups with kids in books at their instructional level—in other words, texts—that if left to their own devices—they could read with 75% comprehension). But when students are required to read texts more likely to be at a frustration level, then you only provide scaffolding on a whole class basis (oh, how I wish you would have described that explicitly). My approach to this is different: when children need a lot of help to carry out a task (such as when asked to read a text that they can’t manage on their own), I think it’s best to provide a lot of close support. And, when they can do reasonably well without me, I try to step back a bit and give them their head. You apparently believe the opposite—you are close by and supportive when they don’t need you, and you are more distant and removed when real and immediate support would be beneficial. That seems backwards. Ultimately, the only thing that matters in this is how well your students can read. If they can successfully read the text levels set by your standards—on their own—then what you are doing sounds great to me. But if many of them can only do such reading successfully—with adequate word recognition and comprehension—when you’re scaffolding for them, then you might want to rethink some of your approaches. Your kids might be growing by “leaps and bounds” (I’d be happy to examine the evidence), but if they aren’t growing sufficiently to reach the standards, then I’d encourage you to be less dedicated to particular instructional approaches and more dedicated to helping your kids reach particular goals. Finally, you requested some research sources. There are many bodies of research that nibble at the edges of this topic, including studies that have challenged the accuracy and reliability of the ways that we identify children’s instructional levels, examined correlationally the relationship between how well students are matched to books and student learning, relationships among text levels and student interest, and the effectiveness of the kind of group instruction that you describe including its impact on various demographic groups like high poverty populations or African American children. Those bodies of research aren’t particularly kind to the instructional level theory, but here I’ll only provide citations of studies that have directly evaluated the effectiveness of teaching students with texts at various challenge levels. I’d gladly include studies that have found instructional level teaching to be more effective; unfortunately, those studies don't exist in the scientific literature. References Baker, R.S.J., D’Mello, S. K., Rodrigo, M.M.T., & Graesser, A.C. (2010). Better to be frustrated than bored: The incidence, persistence, and impact of learners’ cognitive-affective states during interactions with three different computer-based learning environments. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 68, 223–241. Brown, L.T., Moore, K.A.J., Wilcox, B.R., & Barrett, T.S. (2017). The effects of dyad reading and text difficulty on third-graders reading achievement. Journal of Educational Research, 111(5), 541-553. Homan, S.P., Hines, C.V., & Kromrey, J.D. (2010). An investigation of varying reading level placement on achievement of chapter 1 students. Reading Research and Instruction, 33(1), 29-38. Jorgenson, G.W., Klein, N., & Kumar, V.K. (2015). Achievement and behavior correlates of matched levels of student ability and material difficulty. Journal of Educational Research, 71(2), 100-103. Kamil, M. L., & Rauscher, W. C. (1990). Effects of grouping and difficulty of materials on reading achievement. National Reading Conference Yearbook, 39, 121-127. Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Morris, R. D., Morrow, L. M., Woo, D. G., Meisinger, E. B., Sevcik, R, A., Bradley, B. A., & Stahl, S. A. (2006). Teaching children to become fluent and automatic readers. Journal of Literacy Research, 38, 357-387. Lupo, S. M., Tortorelli, L., Invernizzi, M., Ryoo, J. H. (2019). An exploration of text difficulty and knowledge support on adolescents’ comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 44(4), 457-479. Morgan, A., Wilcox, B. R., & Eldredge, J. L. (2000). Effect of difficulty levels on second-grade delayed readers using dyad reading. Journal of Educational Research, 94, 113-119. O’Connor, R. E., Swanson, L. H., & Geraghty, C. (2010). Improvement in reading rate under independent and difficult text levels: Influences on word and comprehension skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 1-19. Powell, W.R., & Dunkeld, C.G. (1971). Validity of the IRI reading levels. Elementary English, 48, 637-642. |
3/1/2015
Dr S. - Please clarify what you mean my the appropriateness of using "instructional level" text in 1st grade. You say, "...it’s important to stress that instructional level appears to matter initially—that’s when kids are first learning to read—but it doesn’t seem to matter after that. Perhaps you are working with first-graders or kids who are reading at a first-grade level, in which case, I think you're going the right direction."
I am guessing you do not mean instructional level in terms of systems such as the F&P A-Z text gradient. Am I correct in thinking you mean "instructional level text" in terms of where students are in the progression of acquiring the knowledge necessary to read? For example, a first grade teacher works with a small group of children who need to learn the CVC syllable pattern. She explicitly teaches this and gives students text that allows ample opportunity to solve CVC words. Then she meets with a different group of students who are solid with the CVC pattern, so she teaches those children the VCe syllable pattern and has them use text that offers ample opportunity to solve VCe words, and so forth, until students have the knowledge they need to access more complex, less decodable texts.
Is that what you mean by instructional level text being appropriate for 1st grade? Thanks!
3/1/2015
The term "instructional level" has been used in the field since the 1940s to refer to how texts are matched to students. The claim has been that texts must be at a students' instructional level if they are going to learn well. Instructional level is determined by how well students can read a text aloud and how well they can comprehend it (typically the instructional level designation is accorded to a text if students can recognize about 95% of its words and answer about 75% of the questions asked about it).
So, yes, I'm talking about text gradient (F&Ms scheme, but also text leveling schemes with stronger research supports like Lexiles). The guided reading claim has been that children need to be taught with books at their instructional levels; however, research does not support such claims beyond a beginning reading level and most states have now adopted standards that specify the levels of text that students must learn to read if they are to meet standards (in other words, if you teach students in Grade 2-12 at their instructional levels instead of the grade levels, you will NOT be teaching your state's standards).
3/1/2015
I guess I am confused about the lack of attention to text decodability in this post. I worry that K/1 teachers in schools that emphasize the F&P text gradient could walk away from this post and think it was best practice to assess a child's instructional level with the F&P benchmark assessment, determine "He's a G," and only instruct that child using books that F&P have stamped with a G - regardless of the texts' ability to help students acquire and apply specific skills that align with current instructional targets.
Would decodability and text-lesson match not be something for K/1 teachers to consider when selecting books for students, especially in the context of explicitly and systematically teaching the Reading Foundational Skills outlined in the CCSS?
See the following for the same idea in an intervention setting: Murray, M. S., Munger, K. A., Hiebert, E. H.(2014). An analysis of two reading intervention programs: How do the words, texts, and programs compare? Elementary School Journal, 114(4), 479-500
Thank you for your time and thoughts.
3/1/2015
"Instructional level" made a bit of sense in the 1940s+ pre-Standards&StandizedTests era when primary reading instruction was conducted with a grade-by-grade textbook and workbook per child.
Then, by Grade 3ish, reading instruction had ended and was morphing into Literature. The protocol worked out (albeit dysfunctionally) because there were no "tests aligned to standards" that teachers had to contend with.
Today, "instructional level" is a figment of imagination and it depends on who is imagining. The teacher's "resources" have expanded from a textbook to a trunk load + "computers." Students at each grade level are strewn "all over the place" "below grade" and "above grade" as well as "at grade."
If a child has learned/been taught how to handle the English Alphabetic Code in interacting with text, "the kid can read"--and will comprehend what is being read as well as if the communication was spoken.
With that capability, individuals can read anything they want to or that EdLand wants them to. If they don't have the requisite background experience they won't comprehend what they are reading, no matter how much "close reading" is crammed into them.
"Instructional levels" and "leveled texts" meet the needs of publishers and professors, but they are cruel and inhumane to everyone else.
Granted, the pubs and profs mean well, but who doesn't.
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2024 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.