Blast from the Past: This entry first posted on June 4, 2022, and reposted on August 17, 2024. Recently, I received a couple of similar inquiries, so thought it might be worth reposting. A quick review of recent research revealed studies focused on the importance of cognitive flexibility in phonics development (Boldrini, et al., 2023; Vadasy, et al., 2023) which suggest to me that adding spelling to the phonics regime may contribute to a stronger understanding of the conditional nature of decoding patterns. Since speech-to-print and print-to-speech are not just reverse processes, grappling with both may contribute to the necessary cognitive flexibility. Also, I found another recent study showing that adding spelling activity to phonics instruction improves decoding (Møller, Mortensen, & Elbro, 2022). The evidence continues to increase in support of adding speech-to-print to the mix.
RELATED: Should We Teach Graphicacy?
Teacher question:
I know you typically don’t talk about specific programs, but I really would like to know your thoughts. I had always wanted more training in a structured literacy program/approach. I always thought Wilson, and specifically OG approaches, were the gold standards. More recently, I began reading about programs labeled as speech to print. Proponents of speech to print methods claim it is much faster to teach kids to read (and spell) than OG based approaches. Is there research to support this? Are these studies comparing programs based on OG (that mainly follow a more print to speech approach) and programs that are more specifically speech to print? Thank you!
Shanahan response:
You’re right that I don’t comment on specific programs. However, I do talk about research on programs or the consistency of certain parts of a program with research.
Let’s start with the claim that Orton-Gillingham (OG) and programs derived from it being the “gold standard.”
To me a gold standard program would consistently result in positive learning outcomes and would outperform competing methods. Outperformance would be demonstrated by direct research comparisons, or by meta-analyses summarizing disparate but relevant studies. Gold standard approaches would result, on average, in more learning.
If OG is the gold standard, it should reliably do better than other explicit decoding programs.
The National Reading Panel (NRP, 2000) analyzed the effectiveness of phonics across 38 studies that evaluated 18 different curricula. Our conclusion was that phonics added a valuable ingredient to literacy teaching, and that programs with explicit systematic phonics outdistanced those that did not.
What about different types of phonics teaching?
We made some of those comparisons, too. Synthetic phonics (reading words from individual letters and sounds), for instance, had higher average effects than analytic phonics (focused on syllables, morphemes, and use of known words as analogies). This difference wasn’t statistically significant, however. That means those different approaches did equally well.
We didn’t compare individual phonics programs with each other.
There were usually only 1 or 2 studies of each program. Not enough evidence for meaningful comparisons of Phonics Programs A and B. An exception was OG. There were enough of those studies to compute a meaningful estimate of effectiveness, it could have been compared with the rest of the set.
I didn’t push for such an analysis because I feared the results would be misleading. OG often failed but usually with severely disabled – hospitalized – populations. It was clear that OG exerted no miracle impact on those learners, but would any other program have done so? It would be impossible to say.
Over 20 years, more research has accumulated, and OG now has merited its very own meta-analysis (Stevens, Austin, Moore, Scammacca, Boucher, & Vaughn, 2021). That study found OG to be effective but with rather modest benefits – lower effectiveness than for the average phonics study that NRP considered.
So much for being the gold standard!
Orton-Gillingham procedures are no more effective than any other explicit systematic phonics instruction – despite the religious fervor of some of its advocates.
Of course, those true believers, argue against these data:
“They didn’t look at the right version of OG.”
“I do it a little differently than others and it really works well for my kids.”
“The newer trainers aren’t as good as the past ones, so they probably studied teachers who weren’t well trained.”
Those complaints aside, there is no reason to think those kinds of things affect OG any more or less than any other program. If it’s that hard to find a potent version, then we shouldn’t expect widespread success.
We lack direct comparisons of OG with other phonics approaches, but it looks like it works about as well any (though often it does not do as well as those).
Which moves us on to the second point – the one about speech to print approaches to phonics.
I’ve oft grumbled about the lack of evaluation of individual features of complex instructional programs. Research may affirm the benefits of a program without revealing its active ingredients.
You’d think with all the interest in phonics there would be many such studies exploring the implications of sound tracing, analytic/synthetic approaches, grapheme-phoneme sequences, inclusion of morphological analysis, decodable text, emphasis on consistency versus flexibility, print-to-speech/speech-to-print approaches, dosage variation, and so on.
Unfortunately, there are few research comparisons of print-to-speech and speech-to-print. There is relevant information about that difference, just nothing definitive yet.
Historically, phonics programs tended to emphasize print-to-speech. Kids are taught to identify letters, to link sounds to those letters, and then to sound out words by sounding each letter. That sequence mirrors the process readers must use during reading: look at the letters and use that information to generate a phonological representation.
It seems reasonable to teach students explicitly what we eventually want them to do. However, advantageous curriculum designs do not necessarily mirror their end points so closely. Engaging in a process like reading and learning to read are not the same thing.
Perhaps the opposite – starting with phonemes and pronunciations and connecting those to letters and printed words – might be a good idea. It’s possible that trying to spell and write words does more to enhance phonemic awareness and it may somehow make the phonology more prominent or easy to perceive (Wasowicz, 2021).
The earliest evidence I know of on this was reported by Jeanne Chall (1967). In her qualitative review of research on phonics, she concluded that programs with spelling, writing, and/or dictation did better than those without.
I followed up on that in my reading-writing relationship research in the 1980s. I found spelling and decoding to be closely related, even when a lot of other variables were available to suck up the variance (Shanahan, 1984). Later, Ginger Berninger and her colleagues followed up on that and with an even more ambitious effort they found the same thing (Berninger, Abbott, Abbott, Graham, & Richards, 2002).
Marilyn Adams (1990) and Linnea Ehri (1997) both theorized on that possibility as well, and Steve Graham reported a meta-analysis on spelling instruction that found spelling to improve reading – probably because of its contribution to decoding (Graham & Santangelo, 2014).
That’s all fascinating, but it’s indirect. It suggests value, it doesn’t prove it.
Not everyone agrees with that assessment. Louisa Moats (1998, 2005, 2010), for example, has several publications that treat this as a settled matter, claiming speech-to-print to be most effective. Her reasoning hasn’t convinced me, and yet the preponderance of current data are on her side. (I suspect that adding speech-to-print is effective including adding spelling to traditional phonics instruction, but it is too early to claim it to be a proven fact).
Perhaps the closest thing we have to a direct test of the proposition is a meta-analysis of 11 studies (Weisler & Mathes, 2011). It concluded that instruction that integrated encoding into decoding instruction led to significantly higher reading achievement. Still not the strongest evidence – because it combined investigations that compared encoding with decoding (Christensen & Bowey, 2005) along with those that compared encoding instruction with things like extra math lessons (Graham, Harris, & Chorzempa, 2002).
At some point – any decoding program must focus on print-to-speech, since that is what we do in reading. However, I think there are real benefits to be derived from activities like invented spelling, spelling instruction, word construction from sounds, and so on – in any phonics program. Speech-to-print activities appear to increase learning. My advice: get a phonics program that includes such activities or layer them into a traditional print-to-speech program (including OG).
READ MORE: Shanahan on Literacy Blogs
References
Adams, M. (1990) Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham, S., & Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(1), 39–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940203500104
Boldrini, G., Fox, A. C., & Savage, R. S. (2023/01//). Flexible phonics: A complementary ‘next generation’ approach for teaching early reading. Literacy, 57(1), 72-86. https://doi.org/10.1111/lit.12308
Chall, J.S. (1967). Reading: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ehri, L. C. (1997). Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell (pp. 237-269). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Graham, S., & Santangelo, T. (2014). Does spelling instruction make students better spellers, readers, and writers? A meta-analytic review. Reading and Writing, 27, 1703-1743. DOI:10.1007/s11145-014-9517-0
Moats, L.C. (1998). Teaching decoding. American Educator, 22(1), 1-9.
Moats, L. C. (2005). How spelling supports reading and why it is more regular and predictable than you may think. American Educator, 29(4), 12-22, 42-43.
Moats, L. C. (2010). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Møller, H. L., Mortensen, J. O., & Elbro, C. (2022). Effects of integrated spelling in phonics instruction for at-risk children in kindergarten. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 38(1), 67-82. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2021.1907638
National Reading Panel (U.S.) & National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U.S.). (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read : an evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
Shanahan, T. (1984). Nature of the reading-writing relation: An exploratory multivariate analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 466–477.
Stevens, E.A., Austin, C., Moore, C., Scammacca, N., Boucher, A.N., & Vaughn, S. (2021). Current state of the evidence: Examining the effects of Orton-Gillingham reading interventions for students with or at risk for word-level reading disabilities. Exceptional Children, 87(4), 397-417.
Vadasy, P. F., Sanders, E. A., & Cartwright, K. B. (2023///Oct 2023 - Dec). Cognitive flexibility in beginning decoding and encoding. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 28(4), 412-438. https://doi.org/10.1080/10824669.2022.2098132
Wasowicz, J. (2021). A speech-to-print approach to teaching reading. LDA Bulletin, 53(2), 10-18.
Weiser, B., & Mathes, P. (2011). Using encoding instruction to improve the reading and spelling performances of elementary students at risk for literacy difficulties: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 17-200.
Click here to read the 37 comments generated by the original version of this entry.
LISTEN TO MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Podcast
Thanks, Tim, for this important reminder about encoding and decoding. My instructional guide to reading, From Sound to Summary: Braiding the Reading Rope to Make Words Make Sense, rests upon Ouellette and Gentry's book, Brain Words: How the Science of Reading Informs Teaching. Here's what they say about the "listening first, spell-to-read approach":
“New information from cognition psychology demonstrates how having a deep level of knowledge of words in the brain—including how to hear them, say them, read them, and spell them correctly—turns out to be a very big deal. Yet for too many reading programs, establishing a dictionary of brain words is either missing or attempted through teaching methods that aren’t scientifically supported. Instruction is all too often piecemeal—failing to integrate this brain connection and neglecting the aural component of written language. For decades schools have given instruction in deep word knowledge—including explicit spelling instruction—very low priority while touting phonological awareness alone, phonics only for isolated decoding lessons, fluency and comprehension as separate entities without capitalizing on their essential deeper connection to word reading and spelling” (p. 5).
This is far too timely to be coincidental.
I've recently reached out to Dr. Mark Seidenberg about this very question, and more broadly about how to appropriately bridge the gap between research and practice. In a correspondence he recommended revisiting your blog (which I had already subscribed to). In brief, I'm a veteran literacy instruction teaching coach at a private school for students with language-based learning disabilities. I'm in a relatively unique position (in coordination with a team of other professionals) to ensure that the school is following best practices in terms of our students' learning needs (first) and also our new teachers' training.
I shared a few concerns with Dr. Seidenberg about troublesome rabbit trails the school may have veered off on, and I'm trying to reach out to respected experts in literacy and research to get back on the right path.
Thank you for sharing this particular blog post again. It is extremely valuable and important to my work.
In an ideal world, I would love to coordinate with you, Dr. Seidenberg, our research coordinator, and the best and brightest upcoming researchers to conduct some studies on the efficacy of our literacy training and approach at the school. It is vital that we get it right because our students desperately need to become successful readers. The parents and guardians spend time, money, and a few tears trying to get their kids into a school that will help them reach their full educational potential.
I would love to continue the conversation. All the best and thank you for what you do!
Paul
I was trained in Wilson and their lesson plans are made up of 3 parts-one is reading the other is spelling. Print to speech (visual) activities are done in the first part, speech to print (auditory) activities are done in the second part. There is also dictation of sounds, words and sentences.
I also trained with the OG Online Academy-there is always a two part process-visual and auditory-decoding and encoding.
Why do people think OG doesn’t include spelling?
You write, Tim, ‘Historically, phonics programs tended to emphasize print-to-speech. Kids are taught to identify letters, to link sounds to those letters, and then to sound out words by sounding each letter. That sequence mirrors the process readers must use during reading: look at the letters and use that information to generate a phonological representation.’ Yes, indeed. I would just add ‘then blending the sounds’ after ‘sounding each letter’. The sounds produced in response to letters are isolated artificial versions of the co-articulated sounds of spoken words, but the blending converts them into those co-articulated sounds.
By contrast, a speech-to-print approach now seems to mean that it’s better for children to start with the spelling routine of segmenting a simple spoken word (e.g. ‘mat’) into its component sounds, selecting a letter for each sound, and arranging the letters in the right order. In videos I’ve seen of this approach, it’s not clear whether or how the children already know the letters.
I’m all in favour of teaching early reading and spelling reversibly, but I don’t see the segmenting-first routine as better, and I think it may actually be harder for beginners, as the co-articulated sounds of spoken words are not easy to separate, particularly if the sounds represented by letters are not known. This is shown by studies of adults who are illiterate or literate only in languages which are not alphabetically written.
As you say, however, we need proper evidence.
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2024 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.