Teacher question:
Our local school district still teaches "sight words." I know that people mean various things when they call words "sight words"-- words that kids don't have the phonics principles for yet, words that are high frequency, and words that "are not decodable." I also understand that brain research says memorizing whole words is a poor practice, and I know that "sight words" is a term that is being phased out in order to communicate that 80% of words are decodable, and emphasizing helping kids flexibly solve words using the parts that do follow predictable phonics rules. Will you please weigh in?
RELATED: My state is banning instructional practices… or, how to look like you are teaching effectively…
Shanahan responds:
I know of no brain research that shows memorizing words to be a bad practice. In fact, we don’t know what information is stored in the brain about words (rules?, patterns?, images of the words themselves?), so memorizing some words could be beneficial to the overall reading process. There certainly is research that shows sight word instruction contributes positively to fluency and comprehension (Griffin & Murtaugh, 2015), and it isn’t clear t what role words themselves play in the development of orthographic mapping -- only that they may play some role (Price?Mohr, & Price, 2018; Schmalz, Marinus, & Castles, 2013).
When it comes to sight vocabulary definitions, I’m in the camp that reserves that label to words the students can recognize seemingly instantaneously. Curricula or instructional intentions play no role in the matter. If a student recognizes a word immediately on sight, then it is a sight word no matter how or why that word was learned.
Think of a sight word as being something akin to your best friend’s name. I’m not especially gifted when it comes to learning names. But I can tell you that my wife, Cyndie, would make life a bit unpleasant around here if I hesitated on her name. Sight words are like your best friends’ names. They are words that you know immediately with no hesitation.
Reading programs may fuzz this definition up a bit. They rather hopefully label words that they designate for direct instruction as sight words, as if the instructional success were a certainty already accomplished. As you point out, they may focus on high frequency words, words that aren’t easily decoded, or words that they simply want to use in their stories. Those are all good reasons for trying to teach some words, but whether those words will become sight vocabulary has more to do with how they are taught or how much time is spent on them.
Naïve observation, behavioral research, and brain study concur that sight vocabulary is about memory (Berglund-Barraza Tian, Basak, & Evans, 2019; Joseph, Nation, & Liversedge, 2013). However, that shared insight leads to very different conclusions about teaching and learning.
Historically, the recognition that young readers benefit from knowing words was translated into graded word lists, flash cards, word drills, and special instructional texts with specific word repetition routines. Psychologists expended much effort trying to determine how many times a student had to see a word before it entered the sight vocabulary: rote repetition was imagined to be the most efficacious approach.
More recent study proposes more nuanced conclusions about what it takes to “memorize” a word.
We can, of course, memorize individual words through brute force paired associate repetition. The issue isn’t whether one can learn words that way, but whether it’s efficient enough for readers to master 40,000 sight words or whether it describes how readers gain the ability to read most words.
Anyone who has carefully monitored young children’s progress in learning to read notices a magical transformation. Initially, learning words seems to be mainly about rote memorization, but for those boys and girls who become readers, new sight words seem to accumulate almost effortlessly.
Something changes.
Students are doing more than remembering more words.
They seem to be learning how to learn words. When children are learning to read, they are learning how to remember words – how to organize them in memory, how to recognize them without decoding or with minimum of decodable effort. Linnea Ehri (2014, 2020) has best described this memory development process.
But if that is the case – and there are many good reasons to think that it is – then it makes sense to try to get words into memory through analysis rather than repetition alone (Hickey, 2007; Newman, Jared, & Haigh, 2012; Steacy, Fuchs, Gilbert, Kearns, Elleman, & Edwards, 2020; Stuart, Masterson, & Dixon, 2000). Memorization often focuses on trivial features (e.g., first letter alone, shape of the word) that may facilitate recall of a small set of words, but which do little to help organize these words into the word reading/spelling system that must develop. There is some evidence that memorized words are stored in memory differently than words that are learned through analysis (Yoncheva, Wise, & McCandliss, 2015), but this appears to be just an immediate effect as word memorization and analysis interact over time making it appear to be a much more fluid and flexible process (Barr, 1984-1975; Biemiller, 1970). [I suspect this Yoncheva study to be what you were referring to. It does show that initially upon learning we store memorized and analyzed words in different parts of the brain, but it says nothing about how words are learned best or what the long-term significance of those differences are. It is important to not try to read too much about learning into such studies.]
I think it makes sense to have students master some words early on – including high frequency words, and those with exceptional spelling patterns. But such word instruction should focus attention on what it is that makes these words unique – their sequences of letters.
This is not outlandish, even with a language like English that has such complex spelling patterns. I’ve long called for first grade teachers to make sure kids can read the 100 most frequent words (and for second grade teachers to do the same with the first 300 words). How would focusing on sound-symbol relations facilitate the learning of these words?
My analysis of the 100 most frequent words (the Fry list) concludes that 55 of these words are entirely decodable – that is, all the orthographic features of these words match the most common pronunciations for letter combinations. Words like and, or, had, but, we, she, up, and will can easily be learned through beginning decoding instruction alone. Another 42 of these frequent words at least partially meet this criterion. Words like the, their, you, were, and his have elements that make them exceptional (that e in the is kind of funky, as is the ou in you, and the s in his is a little strange too), but they also include conventional elements (there is nothing unusual about the th in the, the y in you, or the hi in his).
My advice on attempts to teach sight vocabulary:
1. Provide beginning readers with a substantial decoding program that shows students how to use letters and spelling patterns for reading words.
2. When you are teaching spelling patterns make sure those high frequency words are included in the instruction where relevant (e.g., if you are teaching the consonant digraph th, words like them, these, the, and their should be included in the examples or practice items).
3. Providing a small amount of direct instruction in some key words that you want students to master is very reasonable. (Experience tells me that as little as 5 minutes a day is enough for this part of a reading curriculum).
4. Words that you teach – either directly or through their inclusion in a decoding program – should receive frequent repetition both in isolation and context – I want kids to learn those patterns, of course, but also want them to know those exemplars.
5. Monitor learning to be sure students have mastered those highest frequency words because of their impact on fluency and comprehension (and the role they might play in anchoring decoding skills).
6. When teaching specific words, facilitate word learning by focusing student attention on the letter sequences, spelling, and decodability of the words. Such teaching reduces the numbers of repetitions needed to accomplish learning.
References
Barr, R. (1974-1975). The effect of instruction on pupil reading strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 10(4), 555-582. doi.org/10.2307/747502
Biemiller, A. (1970). The development of the use of graphic and contextual information as children learn to read. Reading Research Quarterly, 6(1), 75-96. doi.org/10.2307/747049
Berglund-Barraza A., Tian, F., Basak C., & Evans J. L. (2019). Word frequency is associated with cognitive effort during verbal working memory: A functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 433. doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00433
Ehri, L. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling memory, and vocabulary learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 5–21. doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.819356
Ehri, L. (2020). The science of learning to read words: A case of systematic phonics instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(1), S45–S60. doi.org/10.1002/rrq.334
Hickey, T. M. (2007). Fluency in reading Irish as L1 or L2: Promoting high-frequency word recognition in emergent readers. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(4), 471-493. doi.org/10.2167/beb455.0
Joseph, H. S. S. L., Nation, K., & Liversedge, S. P. (2013). Using eye movements to investigate word frequency effects in children's sentence reading. School Psychology Review, 42(2), 207-222.
Newman, R. L., Jared, D., & Haigh, C. A. (2012). Does phonology play a role when skilled readers read high-frequency words? evidence from ERPs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(9), 1361-1384. doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.603932
Price?Mohr, R. M., & Price, C. B. (2018). Synthetic phonics and decodable instructional reading texts: How far do these support poor readers? Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 24(2), 190-196. doi.org/10.1002/dys.1581
Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., & Castles, A. (2013). Phonological decoding or direct access? regularity effects in lexical decisions of grade 3 and 4 children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 338-346. doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.711843
Stuart, M., Masterson, J., & Dixon, M. (2000). Spongelike acquisition of sight vocabulary in beginning readers? Journal of Research in Reading, 23(1), 12-27. doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.00099
Steacy, L. M., Fuchs, D., Gilbert, J. K., Kearns, D. M., Elleman, A. M., & Edwards, A. A. (2020). Sight word acquisition in first grade students at risk for reading disabilities: An item-level exploration of the number of exposures required for mastery. Annals of Dyslexia, 70(2), 259-274. doi.org/10.1007/s11881-020-00198-7
Yoncheva Y. N., Wise, J., & McCandliss, B. (2015). Hemispheric specialization for visual words is shaped by attention to sublexical units during initial learning. Brain Language, 145-146, 23-33. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.04.001.
LISTEN TO MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Podcast
F & P calls these words "islands of certainty" for children! I still teach them as well.
I notice in Advice #6 ("When teaching specific words, facilitate word learning by focusing student attention on the letter sequences, spelling, and decodability of the words. Such teaching reduces the numbers of repetitions needed to accomplish learning.") that you don't begin with phonology. No "Say it after me" to introduce the words. Your comment?
Also, would you include set for variability cues to have students brainstorm sound-spelling options?
Thanks, Tim. Uhhh... personal question. You didn't actually climb part way up Everest, did you? With a sherpa?
Mark--
I have no problem having the students saying the word (and sounding the soundable parts) on that step.
Yep, I climbed to Everest Base Camp -- at about 18,000 feet altitude.
tim
I really appreciate the clarity you bring here. The point is to have a vast number of words instantly recognized - including connecting to the words meaning(s) in context. Just like your thoughtful comments on guided reading last week, the term "sight words" now gets us quite confused unnecessarily. I also recommend those top 300 sight words, and focus on teaching the phonics directly within them which gets to thousands more. I have created a handy tool to support analysis by finding these patterns in the Fry list quickly allowing kids to see these patterns in regular and quasi-regular words.
I appreciate also Mark's question and your comment on hearing and saying a word first. When I teach this, I suggest kids isolate the sounds, then represent the sounds as best they can, then correct their initial spelling if incorrect by examining the word comparing to what they wrote, aka analysis. I am particularly interested in differences I am seeing between print to speech vs speech to print and think the latter is a real key to increasing student success. Any comments on speech to print (or expressive to receptive) as a more powerful pathway? Your prior comments and recommendation of Wasowicz's Literacy Learning Network (2021) about the power of writing are echoing here.
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2024 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.