I received two recent letters asking similar questions. Both correspondents noticed that I make a big deal about amount of instruction and they wanted to see the research that I rely on when I encourage schools to maximize time.
Although there are lots of studies of time and its role in student learning —
not just in reading, but in education generally — these studies aren’t always
easy to find. If you look up time or amount of instruction in ERIC, there are
studies, but you’ll miss out on some of the best examples. You probably won’t
find the research on full-day kindergarten there, but are such studies about
anything but amount of instruction (the kids either get 2½ or 5 hours per day
of schooling)? You’ll probably miss out on the homework studies, too, but I
would put them in the increased time basket as well. And where do the analyses
of A Nation at Risk (1983) fit? That was the report that
kicked off our seemingly permanent school reform efforts. They compared the
lengths of school years across nations, and found that kids in the U.S. were
getting less teaching than was available in many of the countries who
outperform us academically (a more recent analysis was in my newspaper this
week… only one country that is doing better than us has a comparable length
school year (Finland); everyone else has more days of schooling. And there are
studies of the time commitment required to develop genius (Walberg, 1981, Gifted
Child Quarterly), or models of what works in school reform (e.g., Fuller
& Clarke, Review of Educational Research, 1994). Most of those
studies won’t show up in a literature search focused on time, but they are
about time, just as are the studies of extended school years, extended school
days, tardiness and absenteeism, afterschool programs, summer school programs,
and efforts to use the school day better (and there are both correlational or
experimental studies on all of those topics).
If you go back a ways, you can find really neat literature reviews showing that
when students engage in generative activities (like writing summaries or
answering questions after reading), they do better than when they just read. It
became a standard rhetorical turn in such reviews to wonder if it was the
activities that conferred the learning advantages or whether the readers in the
experimental conditions were just spending more time on texts because of the
activities. That was seen at the time as a research design problem, and reviews
by Rothkopf, Wittrock, and A. Gagne (all appearing inReview of Educational
Research in the 1970s) treated time as a confound that may have
boosted the experimental groups performance, making it difficult to determine
whether the reading comprehension interventions were really working. None of
those studies would be considered to be time studies, but I would include them
since their benefits came from adding time. Yes, getting students to spend more
time on text helps them to comprehend more — as long as they are engaged in
thinking about the meaning of the text.
There are also “natural experiments” that are evident when a community begins
sending kids to school (such as has happened recently in Egypt or as happened
here in the U.S. since Brown v. Board of Education--African American school attendance
climbed dramatically in the 1950s and 1960s and achievement climbed with it).
During the 20th century, most U.S. states increased the length of the school
year from about 100 days to the current 180: and there were gains in learning
associated with that (by 1970 we were no longer increasing the school year or
including new populations in schooling; 1970 is when our achievement gains
leveled off to where it is now). One can look at education in South Korea over
the past decade or so, to see what increasing a school year and enrollment
percentages can do to achievement levels. (Is anyone surprised that the U.S.
high schools with the highest dropout rates also have the highest absenteeism?
Kids at those schools usually miss more than 30 days a year).
Herb Walberg once told me that there was something like 10,000 studies on the
role of time or amount of teaching in education. That was years ago, and I
suspect the number has grown quite a bit since then. I never checked Herb on
this point (let me see the bibliography, Herb), but experience tells me that he
is usually correct about such claims, so I trust it (especially when I can so
easily find so many studies of time myself). Given that, I think it is pretty
clear that amount of instruction matters.
But there is an important caveat, oft raised during these past few decades:
there is a difference between allotted time and engaged time. That is, learning
is going to come from how time is used and not just the how much time is
available (Jane Stallings wrote a nifty article about this in the Educational
Researcher in 1980, and Barack Rosenshine and David Berliner wrote
similar pieces around the same time). I have seen schools hire teachers to
deliver extra instruction to kids through afterschool programs: instruction
that, ultimately, was not delivered (in some cases, because kids didn’t show up
and in others because the teachers simply didn’t teach). Of course, if the
teacher is presenting something or leading a discussion and the kids aren’t
paying any attention (lots of allotted time, but no engaged time), no learning
is going to happen.
We need to make sure that our schools have enough time to teach reading
effectively, and I suspect we will need longer school years and longer school
days to accomplish that with all kids. But currently we are not using teaching
time well, so simply expanding time availability is not a sufficient answer.
Below I have listed some of the time studies that I use in my courses here at
the university. The Smith paper is available online and it shows how teachers
often fail to use the allotted time in the Chicago Public Schools (there are
similar reports about teaching elsewhere, with sadly similar results). So,
longer school years and school days yes! But let’s make darn sure that we
provide the “bell to bell” teaching that Mel Riddle talks about (Mel was the
principal who turned around a severely challenged high school in Fairfax Co.,
Virginia; one of the big tools he used to raise achievement there was getting
teachers to use the instructional time for —you guessed it— teaching!).
American College Testing. (2006). Reading between the lines. Iowa
City: American College Testing.
Carroll, J.B. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers College
Record, 723–733.
Cooper, H. (2001). Summer school: Research-based recommendations for
policy makers. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and
Improvement.
Cooper, H., Nye, B., Charlton, K., Lindsay, J., & Greathouse, S. (1996).
The effects of summer vacation on achievement test scores: A narrative and
meta-analytic review.Review of Educational Research, 66, 227–268.
Cooper, H., Robinson, J. C., & Pastall, C. A. (2006). Review of
Educational Research,76, 1-62.
Cooper, H. Robinson, J. C., & Patall, E. A. (2006). Does homework improve
academic achievement? A synthesis of research, 1987-2003. Review of
Educational Research, 76(1), 1-62.
Filby, N.N., & Cahen, L.S. (1985). Teacher accessibility and student
attention. In C.W. isher & D.C. Berliner (Eds.), Perspectives on
instructional time (pp. 203–215). New York: Longman.
Frazier, J.A., & Morrison, F.J. (1998). The influence of extended-year
schooling on growth of achievement and perceived competence in early elementary
schooling. Child Development, 69, 495–517.
Frederick, W. C. The use of classroom time in high schools above or below the
median reading score. Urban Education, 11(4), 459-464.
Fusaro, J. A. (1997). The effects of full-day kindergarten on student
achievement: A meta-analysis. Child Study Journal, 27(4), 269-279.
Heyns, B. (1978). Summer learning and the effects of schooling. New
York: Academic Press.
Smith, B.A. (1998). It’s about time: Opportunities to learn in
Chicago’s elementary schools. Chicago: Consortium on Chicago School
Research.
Stallings, J.A., & Mohlmna, G.G. (1982). Effective use of time in
secondary reading classrooms. ERIC Document 216 343.
If you want even more citations, go into Google Scholar and type: engaged time
teaching reading. That’ll give you a half million choices! Or type in some of
the names or articles above and see what else comes up. Happy hunting.
Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!
Copyright © 2024 Shanahan on Literacy. All rights reserved. Web Development by Dog and Rooster, Inc.
Comments
See what others have to say about this topic.