Which Texts for Teaching Reading: Decodable, Predictable, or Controlled Vocabulary?

  • instructional level
  • 09 February, 2019
  • 11 Comments

Teacher question:

I'm looking for help with information or resources about text types for early readers. We have decodable text, text with high-frequency words, and predictive text. It seems like a reasonable strategy to provide our fragile readers with more opportunities to read these low-complexity texts while we shore up issues with phonological awareness. Many teachers over the years have complained to me, an instructional coach, about a lack of available texts to meet the need of students as they proceed through the year and the text complexity increases. Even with popular curriculum programs, teachers usually have very limited options with beginning reader texts, and it isn't clear how the different types are meant to be used or the benefits of each. Do you have any advice for novice teachers about using different text types with our vulnerable readers? 

Shanahan response:

The role of text in reading instruction has always been a big instructional question for parents and teachers—but it has not drawn the same kind of research interest as many other issues.

Nevertheless, the research does provide clues and it suggests that kids are likely to be best off in classrooms that provide them with a mix of these text types rather than a steady diet of any one of them—nor do I see the progression through these as developmental, with kids graduating from one kind of simplified text to another.

Let’s start with the basic premise that when someone is beginning to learn to read (or to learn almost anything else), the teacher is going to need to ease the way a bit; simplifying the process so the learner can actually engage. Every beginning text scheme that has been tried (e.g., controlled vocabulary readers, predictable texts, decodables, language experience stories, words in color, initial teaching alphabet) is not exactly like the texts that we read because it is a simplification made to allow youngsters to get started.

A second premise is that every scheme to simplify a process and to support beginners is somewhat misleading because the simplification is sure to make some important change to the process they are trying to learn. If a youngster is trying to ride a two-wheeler, training wheels might be a great place to start, but those extra wheels mislead these novices with regard to how to balance when riding.

There is nothing particularly unique about the potential negative impacts of simplifications and supports. Doctors who prescribe crutches are always concerned about potential nerve damage from the crutches and from the muscle atrophy that they might promote. Likewise, social policymakers worry over the role welfare plays in discouraging work. Neither group eschews these supports – they are needed – but they make serious efforts to try to avoid the downsides.

Sadly, advocates of various beginning reading schemes usually appear oblivious to the problems their favorite support systems present to beginning readers. They love the fact that the texts they champion allow kids to read early on. But they ignore the fact that their beginning reading texts—like everyone else’s—differ from the actual universe of texts that we read, and the more these texts diverge the greater the danger that they will be misleading to at least some kids.

Controlled vocabulary readers limit texts to a handful of words that are used repeatedly. New words are added gradually. The major approach to learning these words is memorization. Initially, because they start with so few words, these texts sound very stilted, but as kids memorize more and more words, controlled vocabulary texts sound more and more like language.

For instance, consider this sequence of pages from the old Dick and Jane Readers:

pg. 1:          Dick

pg. 2:          Jane

pg. 3:          Dick and Jane

pg. 4:          Dick and Jane run.

pg. 5:          Jane and Dick run.

pg. 6:         Dick runs.

pg. 7:          Jane runs.

pg. 8:          Run, run, run.

Decodable texts on the other hand try to minimize the numbers of words that students won’t be able to decode. Initially, these texts too sound very artificial since the words they include are limited to very few letters and the same letters over and over. They, too, eventually become more like real language as they proceed.

Here is an example of what is meant by decodable based on the old Linguistic Readers. The idea would be to only introduce such a text once the students had some command of the following phonemes /k/, /m/, /f/. /l/, and the phonogram or word base at.

pg. 1           The cat is fat.

pg. 2:          The mat is flat.

pg. 3:          The fat cat sat on the mat.

 

Predictable texts start with more natural sounding language right out of the gate, but instead of requiring the novice readers to rely on memorized words or mastered letter sounds, the readers must depend upon repetition, context, and pictures to guess at words.

 

Here is the beginning of perhaps the most famous of all predictable books, Bill Martin’s Brown Bear, Brown Bear:

pg. 1:          Brown Bear, Brown Bear what do you see?

pg. 2:          I see a yellow duck looking at me.

pg. 3:          Yellow duck, yellow duck what do you see?

pg. 4           I see a red bird looking at me.

All of these texts “work” in terms of getting kids started with reading.

However, each has problems. For instance, controlled vocabulary readers tend to steer kids towards guessing at unknown words based upon the words in their memory. Thus, the child who has memorized when, upon confronting unknown words like which or where, will tend to “read” these, too, as when (Barr, 1975; Biemiller, 1970)—not a very efficient strategy with an alphabetic language.

Decodable texts, too, can be problematic as they tend to steer kids away from meaning, and at times even away from real words. Kids who are used to  strong phonics support and decodable texts tend to try to sound words out more than do other kids (Cheatham & Allor, 2012). But when this doesn’t work (and it doesn’t always work), these kids end up producing nonsense words (mispronunciations based on the sounds they know) or they balk and don’t even read words that they can’t decode easily (Barr, 1975; Biemiller, 1978).

And, predictable texts lead kids to read the pictures instead of the words—not a reading approach at all. In fact, studies show that, since the print isn’t really needed to make sense of many predictable books, the kids learn to ignore the words (Ehri, 1992; Whri & Sweet, 1991; Juel, 1991) and to rely mainly on the context—though such use of context is alien to proficient reading.

Which of these texts should you use?

A basic finding in educational psychology is that simplification or making diverse forms consistent for the purposes of teaching speed acquisition. But they also reduce the learners’ abilities to generalize or transfer these skills to the greater complexity of the actual forms that one needs to learn.

For example, it has been found that providing readers with consistent and simple sound-symbol relations speeds their learning—but when you then ask them to read a more diverse orthography such as the one we use in English, then they are less able to make the needed adjustments (Levin, Baum, & Bostwick, 1963; Levin & Watson, 1963).

If the goal is better beginning reading, this relying heavily on any one of these approaches is pretty smart. If, however, the goal is to teach reading—you know, the kind of reading you and I do—then heavy dependence on any one of these schemes is shortsighted.

Personally—based on my own experiences as a primary grade teacher—I would use all of these kinds of text. My thinking then, and my thinking now, is that the way to prevent someone from being hurt by over dependence on a crutch is to employ a variety of crutches; deriving the benefits of each, while trying to minimize potential damages.

It is very reasonable to employ decodable texts. It gives kids a chance to practice their phonics in a favorable text environment—an environment in which there aren’t likely to be many words that can’t be figured out easily.

But those “experts” who claim that kids should only read such texts for some length of time (e.g., 2-3 years) are just making that stuff up. Research is not particularly supportive of such an artificial text regime (Adams, 2009; Jenkins, et al., 2004; Levin, Baum & Bostwick, 1963; Levin & Watson, 1963; Price-Mohr & Price, 2018). “Teaching children to expect one-to-one consistent mapping of letters to sounds is not an effective way to promote transfer to decoding at later stages in learning to read” (Gibson & Levin, 1975, p. 7).

Please don’t misunderstand where that quote comes from; Eleanor Gibson and Harry Levin were big explicit phonics proponents in their day, but they also believed in following the research.

Those who pushback against any who would dare to present anything other than decodables text to kids often complain that anything else is too hard or discouraging to kids. But that’s where those other text simplifications—that have their own problems—come in.

Having young students reading both simple decodable texts along with controlled vocabulary readers keeps them from being overwhelmed by difficulty—but also prevents them from trying to depend upon memory or simple decoding so much that these approaches do damage.

I’m not a big fan of predictable text in this equation, because it discourages kids from looking at the words. However, even these texts are okay for very brief times. In my classrooms, kids worked with these kinds of texts once a week or less—along with the basal readers, linguistic readers, and language experiences stories that made up the lion’s share of their reading. Predictable texts are fun, they allow a level of early success unmatched by the other texts and they do encourage kids to try to keep reading meaningful and fluent; nothing wrong with any of that.

For a long time, I’ve advocated for substantial amounts of instructional time devoted to decoding, fluency, comprehension, and writing. Decodable texts can be an important part of the decoding instruction, but I’d make controlled vocabulary readers the base of my reading comprehension instruction. Predictable texts can be a lot of fun, too, once in a while.

Comments

See what others have to say about this topic.

Chris Oct 29, 2019 08:30 AM

Hi Tim,

Interesting reading here. I agree that a varied diet isn’t a bad thing for early readers. I do wish, however that there was a larger body of research on the topic of which books to use when.

Here in Australia, there is currently a shift from a more ‘whole language ‘ approach, to one that more closely aligns with research but schools are still filled with predictable texts. Teachers will often deliver an explicit phonics lesson at 9am and then do guided reading with a predictable text at 9.30. It seems so counterproductive.

When I have taught in the US and the UK, the texts used have been far better matched to the skills that the students were learning and thus they seemed to ‘get going’ with reading far more quickly.

I guess there is a need for greater research but also greater reflection from teachers about why we do the things we do.

Cheers,

Chris

Corrine Mar 06, 2020 01:22 AM

This is officially my new favorite article. Once I learned how to teach using OG, I felt conflicted teaching Fountas and Pinnell. But, to be perfectly honest, I actually enjoy teaching this program too. I wish the early guided reading books had more decodeable words, but the kids love these books and enjoy reading them every day. I also see value in the interactive reading component, the shared reading, and the mini-lesson. I teach OG for the phonics portion and I feel, for the first time in my nearly 20 year career, like I am finally getting it right. It is so nice to hear an expert who advocates for a moderate approach.

Thanks so much.

Dana Oct 16, 2021 11:18 PM

Can you provide some modern day examples of controlled vocabulary readers?

Julia S Oct 22, 2020 02:14 PM

Who are the "experts" saying that kids should be working just with decodable readers for 2 to 3 years? I'm a big fan of starting kids on decodable readers, but that seems like an awfully long time. It seems to me that the transition to regular books should happen in the first year. Maybe it takes that long if teachers are going with that "letter of the week" nonsense.

Grace Vyduna-Haskins Feb 14, 2021 09:01 PM

My experience was using decodable text until children had mastered short vowel words and started long vowels. By that time they seemed to adjust quickly to all the words in our basal reader series. For most, that happened mid-year in first grade.

Lara M May 09, 2022 03:28 PM

Thanks for sharing your thoughts. I noticed you wrote you would make controlled vocabulary readers the base of your comprehension instruction. Can you elaborate why?

Timothy Shanahan May 09, 2022 05:22 PM

Lara--
Beginning reading texts need to be relatively easy -- with recognizable patterns and lots of word repetition. However, any simplification system seems to be a bit misleading to kids (at least for a while). Too much emphasis on regularity of spelling patterns leads kids into lifetime biases in spelling and pronunciation that do not match the complexity of the orthographic system. Likewise, controlled vocabulary kinds of readers tend to skew kids towards ignoring spelling patterns in favor of more general cues (like length, shape, first letter). I think it makes sense to use decodable texts within a decoding program to give kids concentrated practice with decoding using the patterns being taught -- but then want kids to see text simplified some other way to discourage them from overdoing it. To me, given the various choices decodability and vocabulary controlled texts are a nice combination that should be mutually supportive without getting kids to over learn how we have simplified the system. Controlled vocabulary can make texts somewhat artificial (as is true of decodables), the benefit is that they do this in different but overlapping ways.

tim

claire knight May 14, 2022 07:43 PM

I am fascinated by the conversations around how long we want students to be on a decodable text and the evidence to support this. Many of the programs advocate that we move the students to authentic texts only after 2 years because what you can read you can write, thus a slower but deeper pace. I guess I am wondering about students who just get it (natural readers or faster learners), as well as those who are able to self teach after some code has been taught.
Another one of my biggest questions from teachers is what do we do with those students who can read at a much higher level than they can write because of not having been taught the code? Any of your pearls of wisdom would be appreciated.

Timothy Shanahan May 14, 2022 08:06 PM

Claire--

Research has not shown decodables to give a boost to children learning and there are serious concerns about providing children a steady dose of text that is more consistent than English. In most cases, no matter how students are taught, they read better than write.

tim

William Feerick Feb 21, 2024 05:05 PM

What are some controlled vocabulary readers that I can use in my first grade classroom?

Timothy Shanahan Feb 22, 2024 08:21 PM

William--

Why not start here?

https://textproject.org/teach-your-child-lessons-beginningreads/

tim

What Are your thoughts?

Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!

Comment *
Name*
Email*
Website
Comments

Which Texts for Teaching Reading: Decodable, Predictable, or Controlled Vocabulary?

11 comments

One of the world’s premier literacy educators.

He studies reading and writing across all ages and abilities. Feel free to contact him.